Transcript LT2Ch3a
Experimental Evidence
Rats drink little saccharin water at
first but increase over time.
Loud tones (110 db) produce
different responses depending on
the background noise (60 vs 80 db).
Habituation occurred at 60 db
Sensitization occurred at 80 db
A loud background is arousing, leading
to greater reactivity, not less.
Conditions Producing Change
More intense (stronger) stimuli
produce stronger sensitization, less
likely to produce habituation.
Greater sensitization and
habituation occur when the stimulus
is repeated frequently.
Changes in the stimulus prevent
habituation.
Turkeys respond to shape changes.
Conditions (Cont.)
Sensitization can occur to many
kinds of stimuli but habituation
occurs only with innate responses.
Habituation and sensitization are
transient (go away after seconds or
minutes between stimuli).
Except long-term habituation.
Dishabituation – response returns
when a sensitizing stimulus occurs.
Opponent-Process Theory
An explanation for addictions.
All experiences produce an affective
reaction (pleasant or unpleasant) –
called the A state.
This reaction gives rise to its
opposite – called the B state.
B state is less intense and lasts longer.
Over time, the A state diminishes
and the B state increases.
The Addiction Process
Tolerance – diminished A state.
Withdrawal – increased B state.
Addictive behavior is a coping
response to the change in B state.
People try to enhance A state to offset
the unpleasantness of the B state.
Without withdrawal symptoms there is
no addictive behavior.
Time prevents B state strengthening.
What Sustains Addiction?
The B state is a non-specific
aversive feeling.
Anything similarly aversive will
motivate the addictive behavior, even if
it has no relation to the substance.
Daily life stress produces a B state that
results in behavior to create an A state.
Parachute jumpers – create a B
state in order to feel the A state.
PSY402
Theories of Learning
Chapter 3
Political Classical Conditioning
Examples of Conditioning
Popcorn at the movies.
Fear of flying -- stronger with more
turbulence (a stronger UCS).
An antelope shying away from low
tree branches.
Nausea at the smell of alcohol after
a hangover.
Conditioned Emotional Responses
Fear is an anticipatory pain
response based on past experience.
Fear is conditioned whenever a CS
is associated with an aversive
(painful or negative) event.
Fear motivates two responses:
Escape (when pain is present)
Avoidance (when pain is imminent)
Conditioning Situations
Sign-tracking (autoshaping) –
animals must recognize signs of
food (UCS) and respond (UCR).
Pigeons pecking at key.
UCR, not an operant response, because
behavior is specific to the stimulus.
Eyeblink conditioning
UCR is rapid, CR is slow.
Many trials are needed (100 pairings)
Fear conditioning
Avoidance is not a good measure of
fear.
Suppression of an operant behavior
occurs with a feared stimulus.
First – an operant behavior is learned.
Second – a CS is paired with an
aversive UCS.
Third – the CS is presented in the
operant chamber.
Suppression Ratio
During CS
SuppressionRatio =
During CS + Without CS
The amount of time during and without the
CS is equal.
The more fear, the lower the suppression
ratio.
Ratios typically fall between 0 and .5
Flavor Aversion Learning
Garcia – rats will not drink water
with saccharin if they get ill after
drinking.
Significant avoidance occurs after just
one trial.
Human food aversions are related
to illness (89%).
Even if illness occurs hours later it is
linked to the previous meal.
Not cognitive – know food not to blame
Conditions Affecting Acquisition
Chapter 3, pages 48-69
Conditioning Paradigms
Delayed conditioning – the CS onset
precedes the UCS onset.
Trace conditioning – the CS starts
and ends before the UCS onset.
Simultaneous conditioning – the CS
and UCS occur together.
Backward conditioning – the UCS
starts and ends before the CS
onset.
Temporal Conditioning
No CS is presented.
The UCS occurs at regular intervals.
A CR eventually occurs just before
the UCS.
Mechanism: a biological state
typically provides the CS.
Waking up just before the alarm goes
off.
Importance of Timing
A cue (CS) needs to be a good
predictor of the UCS.
Optimal inter-stimulus-interval (ISI)
varies with the kind of response.
The latency to respond is different for
different reflexes (saliva, heart rate)
Too long or too short intervals result in
weaker conditioning.
Intermediate CS’s form a bridge.
Importance of CS Strength
CS intensity affects CR strength
only when the CS intensity varies.
If the CS strength is always the same,
then the CS strength doesn’t affect the
size of the CR.
Both dogs must bite in order for their
size to matter.
In order for CS intensity to matter,
it must signal something.
Importance of UCS Strength
Strength of the CR increases with
strength of the UCS.
The more intense the air puff, the
stronger the eyeblink CR.
Salience of the CS
Salience refers to how noticeable a
stimulus is – how likely an organism
is to notice it in the environment.
Preparedness (evolutionary
predisposition) makes some CS
more salient than others.
The more salient the CS, the
stronger the CR and faster learning.
Predictiveness of the CS
Predictiveness refers to how reliably
the CS is associated with the UCS.
When two or more CS’s are present,
only the most reliable elicits a CR.
When the CS occurs with the UCS
more often than the UCS occurs
alone, conditioning occurs.
A CS alone weakens conditioning.
Blocking
Presence of a previously conditioned
CS (existing predictive cue)
prevents conditioning of a new CS.
Parent threats – presence of fear of the
parent prevents acquisition of fear to
another stimulus.
Implications for Parenting
Threats (CS) should reliably be
accompanied by punishment (UCS)
or they will be ignored.
Timing of threat (CS) and
punishment (UCS) should be close
together – not wait until Dad gets
home.
Fear of parents (CER) may block
conditioning of any other CS.