Transcript File
Six substages of the sensorimotor
stage
Reflexes
Primary circular reactions
Secondary circular reactions
Coordination phase
Tertiary circular reactions
Symbolic problem solving
Reflexes substage
First month of life
First display of assimilation and accommodation
Basic cognitive schemas are formed (e.g., sucking
on objects is pleasurable)
Imitate facial gestures, but only in the first
month—may be an attempt to elicit social contact
& ensure survival (reflexive smiling)
Newborn is completely tied to immediate
present—no memory or planning for future
Primary circular reactions
Ages 1-4 months
First nonreflexive schemas emerge as baby
discovers by chance that responses they make are
satisfying and worthy of repetition
These acts are always centered around infant’s
own body
Example: making cooing sounds, sucking thumbs
Secondary circular reactions
4-8 months
Infants discover by chance that they can make
interesting things happen to external objects
(make rubber duck quack)
These reactions are repeated because they’re
pleasurable.
Piaget thought secondary circular reactions
represented the infant’s sense of his own
boundaries—realizes that he’s separate from the
rest of the world
First beginnings of object permanence
Coordination phase
8-12 months
Infant shows truly planned responding to solve
simple problems
First display of voluntary imitation of novel
responses
Can coordinate different senses (smell, sight,
touch, etc.) to solve a problem
Example: lifting a pillow to retrieve pacifier
Tertiary circular reactions
12-18 months
Infants start to experiment with objects to
see what will happen
Lots of trial and error as infant discovers
on his own what works to create a desired
response and what doesn’t
Emergence of true curiosity
Symbolic problem solving
18-24 months
Children begin to internalize their behavioral
schemas to construct mental symbols or images
Capable of inner experimentation—solving
problems mentally without overt trial-and-error
Uses symbols to represent real objects, and
recognizes that the symbol is different from the
real object
Capable of make-believe play and deferred
imitation
Object permanence
The understanding that objects continue to exist
when out of sight
Unclear when this develops. A 1-4 month-old
doesn’t have it; a 4-8 month old will look for
partially concealed objects but not totally
concealed ones.
Babies between 8-12 months still do NOT have a
complete grasp of object permanence. They
make errors.
A-not B-error
Between 8-12 months
A child looks for the object where it was last
found, not where it was last seen.
Child acts as if his own behavior determines
where the object will be found (thinks that
because she found it in place A the first time, it
will still be there, even though she saw her
mother put it under place B)
Object permanence between 1218 months of age
Clear improvement in this age range
Infant will look for objects where they
were last seen; they don’t make the A-not
B-error
Still don’t understand invisible
displacements (if the toy goes completely
out of view, then they’ll look for it where
they last saw it…e.g, in your hand)
Deferred Imitation
The ability to reproduce a previously seen action
at some later time.
Piaget loved this concept; he saw it as a highly
adaptive activity
He thought this was only present during the last
substage of sensorimotor phase (symbolic
problem solving)
Actually, it seems to be present at 6 weeks of age
in a primitive form.
Child isn’t proficient at it until 14 months or so.
Learning
Recall the three types of learning from
section 1
Classical conditioning
Operant conditioning
Observational learning (modeling)
Memory
It’s now known that infants (3 months old) can
remember a variety of complex associations and
visual sequences; these memories can last for 3
months and even up to 9 months or more.
Studies show that very long-term retention of
memories is the exception, not the rule.
The nature of infant memories is highly
controversial.
Rovee-Collier’s paradigm (1987,
2004)
Baby placed in a crib underneath an elaborate
mobile; ankle is tied to the mobile so that when
the baby kicks, the mobile moves.
Baby comes back weeks later with foot untied;
question is: Will the baby kick?
Findings: Baby will kick but only if the mobile is
exactly the same as it was before.
Rovee-Collier says that infants as young as 2
months can remember some experiences through
1 ½ to 2 years of age.
Critics of Rovee-Collier
Critics say the babies are showing only implicit
memory (memory of skills and routine
procedures that are performed automatically; no
conscious recollection) not explicit memory
(conscious memory of facts and experiences).
Most researchers find that babies do not have
explicit memory until the 2nd half of the first year;
it improves dramatically during the 2nd year of
life.
Most infant memories are fragile and short-lived,
except for memories of perceptual-motor actions.
Infantile amnesia
The inability to remember events that occurred
before the age of 2-3
Highly debated; some researchers claim that
people remember birth and intrauterine
experiences. No scientific evidence for this.
Could be because the prefrontal lobes aren’t
matured enough to store memories properly.
Could have to do with language—before the age
of 2-3, we don’t have enough language skills to
store memories.
Autobiographical memories
Memories of your special, one-time events in
your own life that have personal meaning for you
Must have a well-developed sense of self for
autobiographical memories to be stored.
Self-awareness doesn’t occur until 18-24 months,
which is the earliest time that scientists believe
autobiographical events can be recalled.
Must be able to integrate personal experiences
into a meaningful life story
Adult must talk to child about the event to
solidify the memory.
Infant Intelligence Tests
Best-known test is the Bayley Scales of Infant
Development, for children between 1 month and
3 ½ years.
Includes a Mental Scale and Motor scale, and you
get a score similar to IQ score.
Correlates poorly with adult IQ; variations of 1020 points or more
IQ is NOT a stable inborn ability, although it is
strongly genetically influenced. It changes with
experience and age.
Infant Intelligence Tests cont.
It’s not easy to get scores (called “developmental
quotients”) that reflect an infant/toddler’s true
ability. Child is often distracted, bored, or tired.
They predict better for extremely low-scoring
babies.
Today, they’re used mainly for identifying
children whose very low scores may mean they
have developmental delays and need early
intervention.
Piaget’s object permanence tasks predict later IQ
better than “infant IQ” tests.
Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence
Increasingly being used since the 1990s
Focuses on infant’s ability to process information
(e.g., encoding attributes of objects, detecting
similarities/differences in objects).
Uses the amount of time babies take to look at a
new object vs. familiar one to estimate their
intelligence.
Fagan test IS correlated with measures of
intelligence in older children.
Habituation vs. dishabituation
Evidence is mounting that measures of
habituation & dishabituation (recovery of a
habituated response after a change in stimulation)
predict intelligence in childhood and adolescence.
Quicker habituation and greater amounts of
looking in dishabituation reflect more efficient
information processing.
Average correlation between measures at 3-12
months and measures in childhood &
adolescence: .37.