Transcript Powerpoint

Lynx-Hare Cycle Assumptions
• N1 and N2 dependent only on
each other
• predator can find and consume
prey at any prey density
• no Allee effect for predator or prey
at low densities
Variations in predator – prey interactions
could be due to:
--how well predators can find and consume prey
--how well prey can avoid predation
--whether or not a refugium exists where prey can be
safe from predators
Refugium may exist: place for prey to survive
without presence of predator
e.g., prickly pear cactus introduced to Australia
In 1830s for hedge rows, gardens
Also resisted drought and had no predators, so spread
rapidly and became a nuisance species
Cactus moth introduced in 1926, quickly
spread and helped control cactus
Cactus still present in refugia and
expand from them until moth
population resurges
jaeger
Snowy owl
Short-eared owl
Numerical Response
Response of predatory birds to different densities of the brown lemming near
Barrow, Alaska
1951
1952
1953
Brown lemming
1 to 5
Pomarine jaeger
Uncommon, no
breeding
Breeding pairs 4 Mi2
Breeding pairs
18 Mi2
Snowy owl
Scarce, no breeding
Breeding pairs 0.2 to
0.5 Mi2 many
nonbreeders
Breeding pairs 0.2 to
0.5 Mi2 few
nonbreeders
Short-eared owl
Absent
Source: Pitelka et al. 1955.
15 to 20
One record
70 to 80 (ind per acre)
Breeding pairs 3-4 Mi2
Charles Krebs: detailed analysis of Lynx-Hare Cycle
• Disease and parasites?
studied parasite loads in hares for years
none caused direct mortality
• Quality of Food?
measured winter food abundance
hares only eat 20-40% of what’s available
food addition experiments showed
initial growth response, then decline
• Predation?
marked hares
95% mortality due to predation by
lynx, owls, coyotes
exclusion showed high survival rate
Why are cycles synchronous across
large areas and only in high latitudes?
• predators tend to disperse over large areas,
e.g., one marked lynx moved 1100 km, owls
forage over large areas
• more seasonal variations at high latitudes
may drive higher oscillations in cycles there
• get some cycles at lower latitudes, but not
as obvious
Optimal Foraging Theory
• developed by MacArthur and Pianka (1966)
• predicts minimal foraging effort for
maximum energy gain
• minimal effort means lowest search
and handling time
• if food patchy, predators should select
best patches, lowest S&H time
Glimcher (2002)
Krebs Experiment: Bird Foraging over Conveyor Belt
In Krebs' experiment, hungry birds stand over a conveyor belt. An experimenter places
mealworm segments of two sizes on the belt in a pseudorandom sequence. The bird
faces a serial decision problem; it must decide which segments to eat and which to
ignore.
The decisions the bird makes are influenced by the mean rates at which both prey types
are encountered, the difficulty of capturing and eating the segments, and the relative
values of the two different size pieces.
Optimal Foraging in the Wild
whelks
Northwestern Crow
http://www.asnailsodyssey.com
Why the world is green:
1. Insects and other herbivores controlled
by their predators and parasites
Hairston, Slobodkin, and Smith (HSS Model)
2. Not all plants are edible
Nitrogen content of typical plant:
Stem
Leaf
Seed
0.002 %
1-5 % (folivores)
10+ % (granivores)
Also, as plant grows from sprout to mature,
water and protein content declines
Cellulose and lignin increase, need bacteria
and protozoa in gut to digest
Plant defenses from herbivory:
1. Morphological
--thick leaves, thorns, needles
2. Chemical
--secondary compounds
Secondary Compounds
Chemicals produced by plants solely for defense
Take considerable energy to produce
1. Nitrogen compounds
--derived from amino acids
--alkaloids include nicotine, morphine
--mostly toxic, bitter tasting
2. Terpenoids
--oils and resins
--mostly bitter tasting
--includes terpetines, solvents
3. Phenolics
--tannins that hinder digestion
--used in dyes, tanning, inks
Allelopathy: plants use of secondary compounds for
defense against competition from other plants
--can affect growth and development of other plants
around them
Corn plants: use terpenoids to attract a parasitic
wasp, lays eggs in caterpillar feeding on plant
Ethnobiology: study of anthropology and biology,
how humans, past and present, used or use plant and
animal resources in their culture
Herbivore response to plants:
1. Morphological
--teeth, gut
2. Behavioral
--detoxify secondary compounds
Hindgut fermentation
enlarged cecum
Foregut fermentation
enlarged
stomach
Behavioral responses:
1. Eat clay to detoxify compounds
--primates, parrots
2. Coprophagy
-- rabbits
A specialized diet on plants is
not without costs:
1. Red tree vole, eats only conifer needles
--high in tannins, thick cellulose
--slow metabolism, growth
2. Lynx-hare cycle
--browsing by hares stimulates plants
to produce secondary compounds
--food becomes less digestible
--hare numbers decline
--populations cycle without predator
Avoiding Predation
• cryptic and warning coloration
• Batesian mimicry
• Müllerian mimicry
katydid
Cryptic coloration
walking stick
Biston betularia
Warning coloration
Batesian Mimicry
Monarch
Viceroy
coral snake
king snake
Red on yellow, harm a fellow
Red on black, friend of jack
Müllerian
Batesian
Mimicry in Plants
Heliconius butterflies and eggs on
Passion flower leaves
Predators can be specialists or generalists
Specialist versus generalist relates to optimal
foraging theory as well
-- generalist flowers with small nectar rewards usually
are common species
-- specialist flowers with large nectar rewards usually
are rare species
-- a specialist predator seeks out rarer plants with large
rewards rather than waste time getting small
rewards from abundant plants
-- ensures pollination of rare plant