Kin selection
Download
Report
Transcript Kin selection
大學部 生態學與保育生物學學程 (必選)
2010 年 秋冬
團體生活,利他和合作
(Group Living, Altruism, and
Cooperation)
─動物行為學 (Ethology)
鄭先祐(Ayo)
國立 臺南大學 環境與生態學院
生態科學與技術學系 教授
Ayo NUTN Web: http://myweb.nutn.edu.tw/~hycheng/
Part 3. 個體間的互動
生殖行為 (Reproductive Behavior)
親代照顧與交配體系 (Parental Care and Mating
Systems)
溝通:管道與功能 (Communication: Channels and
Functions)
溝通的演化 (The Evolution of Communication)
衝突 (Conflict)
團體生活,利他和合作 (Group Living, Altruism,
and Cooperation)
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
2
17 團體生活,利他和合作
Living in Groups: from aggregation to structured
societies
Benefits of Group living
Costs of living in groups
Balancing costs and benefits
The puzzle of altruism
Individual selection and altruism
Kin selection
Reciprocal altruism
manipulation
Examples of cooperation among animals
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
3
An introduction to group living
Ladybug beetles overwinter in aggregations of
thousands of individuals
They crawl inside rock crevices and houses for shelter
They were introduced from Asia to control invasive
pests
Have become wildly successful and their numbers have
soared
They outcompete and even eat native ladybug species
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
4
Animal groups: simple to complex
Ladybug groups are at the “simpler” end of the spectrum
No evidence that ladybugs interact in complex ways
Other species form groups
Individuals respond to the same features of the
environment
The other end of the scale of animal groups:
chimpanzees
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
5
After conflict, one chimpanzee opponent approaches the
other, offering an open hand
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
6
Benefits of living in groups
Societies: chimpanzees and other species are
members of structured groups
Fitness benefits accrue to animals living in groups
Improved foraging
Decreased risk of predation
Conservation of water and heat
Decreased energetic costs of movement
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
7
Cooperative hunting improves foraging
Cooperative hunting in groups improves foraging success
Harris’s hawks live and hunt in family groups
In early morning, family members gather at one perch site
The group splits into smaller subgroups
The subgroups take turns flying through their family’s area
Hawks employ different hunting tactics
The surprise pounce(猛撲): hawks converge(聚集) on a
rabbit
Flush-and-ambush tactic: hawks flush the rabbit and other
family members pounce on it
Relay (換班) attack: family members chase the prey, with a
new lead bird taking over
each time
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
8
Cooperative hunters share prey
For cooperative hunting to be favored by selection
Individuals must average at least the same amount of food
they would get by hunting alone
Hunting success must be increased in groups
Harris's hawks
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
9
In Harris's hawks, larger group size increases the chance of a kill
Even with more individuals, the energy intake per individual from prey
is higher in larger groups
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
10
Improved foraging: information sharing
Animals improve foraging in groups by information
sharing
Individuals pay attention when conspecifics discover food
They use this information to guide their own foraging
Geese land nearer artificial geese with their heads down in a
feeding position than to those standing erect
Communal roosts or colonies can act as information
centers
Successful foragers return to the roost or colony and then
return to the food site
Seen in cliff swallows and honeybees
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
11
cliff swallows
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
12
Decreasing predation risk
The many eyes hypothesis: better predator detection
Animals in groups give alarm signals
The dilution effect: an individual in a group has a
smaller chance of becoming the next victim
The selfish herd hypothesis: animals in the center of
the group have a lower chance of being preyed upon
Confusion effect: fleeing in different directions
decreases a predator’s ability to track and kill any one
individual
Group members can band together to drive a predator
away
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
13
Conserving heat and water
By huddling together, animals reduce the surface area
exposed to the environment
Reducing the loss of heat or water
Many mammals sleep or overwinter together in
communal burrows
Many birds perch snuggled (偎依) up next to one
another
The metabolic rates of penguins is reduced compared to
isolated birds
Even some slugs (蛞蝓) rest in contact with one
another to reduce water loss
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
14
Conserving energy by moving together
Drafting (riding close behind in the slipstream of
another)
Reduces the amount of energy needed to move
Holds true for animal groups that travel together
Schools of fish
Flocks of birds
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
15
Pelicans (鵜鶘)
flying in a V
formation have
lower heart rates
than birds flying
solo
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
16
Cost of group living: increased
competition
Individuals that live in groups often compete with each
other For mates, nest sites, or food
Some snails secret a mucous net that floats on water
The snail draws the net in to eat the mucous and its catch
When snails group, their mucous nets overlap and stick
Snails adjust for neighbors by retracting their nets quickly
Group-living animals might lose food to thieves
Stealing can be considered a strategy
There is a limit on how many “scroungers” (thieves) a
population supports before the strategy becomes
unrewarding
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
17
Costs of group living: increased disease
and parasites
Proximity to conspecifics presents an increased risk of
infection
Larger colonies of cliff swallows have more bloodsucking swallow bugs
Which reduce nestling mass, and decrease survival
Larger colonies have higher levels of glucocorticoid
hormones, released in response to stress
The stress response was caused by exposure to the
parasites
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
18
Fending off(擊退) disease and parasites
Group-living species have behaviors that help fend off
disease and parasites
Allogrooming: animals groom each other and pick off
ectoparasites
Social insects (honeybees, ants and termites) remove
corpses and other waste from the colony
And wall off or remove infected individuals
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
19
Costs of group living: interference with
reproduction
Extra-pair copulations (outside of the pair bond) are
common Even among “monogamous” birds
Males may take care for young that are not theirs
A female may not notice when another female deposits an
egg in her nest
Allonursing: mammalian mothers nurse offspring not
their own
In shared roosts, reproducing communally or in a small
space
May provide fitness benefits to the nursing mother (e.g.,
feeding related young or the mother will reciprocate later)
It may be misdirectedAyoparental
care (mistaken identity)
教材 (動物行為學 2010)
20
Balancing costs and benefits
Hunting success alone does not explain formation of
prides in lions
Benefits of group living include defense of food, young,
and space against conspecifics
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
21
Costs and benefits of group living may
differ across individuals
Whirligig beetles form aggregations on the surface of
fresh water
Beetles at the outside edge are more likely to get food
But are also more likely to get attacked
Beetles weigh the tradeoff between predation risk and
food availability
Depending on how hungry they are
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
22
Hungry beetles move to the outside of the group, in
spite of the risk
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
23
The puzzle of altruism
Why do some animals help other members of their
species?
A Belding’s ground squirrel increases its risk of being
spotted by an approaching predator when it barks an alarm
Dwarf mongooses feed young of others and guard the den
Eusocial insects (ants, termites, wasps and bees) care for
their colony and rear young that are not their own
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
24
Wild turkey males form coalitions (聯盟) that court
females and defend(保護) them, but only dominant
males father offspring
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
25
Altruism
Altruistic behavior appears to be costly to the altruist
And beneficial to another member of its species
Altruism is defined in terms of fitness
It is a behavior that raises the fitness (number of offspring
produced that live to breed) of another individual
At the expense of the altruist’s direct fitness, measured by
the number of offspring it leaves
How could altruism evolve?
Shouldn’t alleles that promote selfish behavior multiply
more quickly than alleles that promote altruism?
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
26
Individual selection and altruism
Behavior may appear to benefit others at a cost to the actor
In fact, the actor might directly benefit from its behavior
An animal that gives an alarm call may appear to be alerting
others at is own expense
But it improves its own survival by alerting the predator that it
has been seen
Some cichlid fish (慈鯛魚) adopt unrelated young into their
own brood
The parents gain: the adopted young reduce the predation risk
of their own young
Before assuming that a behavior is truly altruistic
Determine if the actor benefits
directly
from the behavior
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學
2010)
27
Kin selection
Natural selection increases the frequency of an altruistic
trait
Individuals improve their fitness through their own offspring
(descendent kin)
And through the reproductive success of other relatives
(nondescendent kin)
Kin selection: family members other than offspring possess
copies of some of the same alleles
Because they inherited the alleles from the same ancestor
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
28
Kin selection duplicates alleles
If family members increase their reproductive success
The alleles that the altruist has in common with them are
also duplicated
Just as if the altruist reproduced personally
Not all relatives have the same likelihood of sharing
alleles
Closer relatives (i.e. siblings) share more alleles than
distant relatives (i.e. cousins)
Coefficient of relatedness (r): the probability that
particular pairs of relatives share the same allele through
common descent
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
29
The coefficient of relatedness
There is a 50–50 chance (probability of 0.5) that any
particular allele will be found in an egg or sperm produced
by the parent
A parent and offspring have a coefficient of relatedness of 0.5
The value of r ranges from 0 (nonrelatives) to 1 (identical
twins or clones)
A family tree can be used to calculate the coefficient of
relatedness between more distant relatives
An animal shares 50% of its genes with a full sibling (r = 0.5)
25% with a half sibling or grandparent (r = 0.25)
Only 12.5% with a first cousin (r = 0.125)
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
30
Calculating relatedness (r): a family tree
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
31
Coefficient of relatedness affects
altruism
When will the gene for an altruistic behavior increase?
Hamilton’s rule: B 1
C r
B = the benefit to the recipient: extra offspring that the
relative produces because of the altruistic act
C = the cost to the actor: the number of offspring it
does not produce because of the altruistic act
r = the coefficient of relatedness between the recipient
and the actor
1/r = a value of 1 or greater (r ranges from 0-1)
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
32
Applying Hamilton’s rule
When should an individual forgo (放棄) reproduction
to help its sister?
For siblings, r = 0.5, so 1/r = 2
The benefits of acting altruistically must outweigh the
costs by 2:1 for an individual to help its sister reproduce
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
33
Stop and think
A bird would have two offspring without help
If her altruistic sister helps her by bringing food to the
nest and driving off predators, she will have five
offspring
This behavior has a cost to the altruist
The altruist will not have any of her own offspring
Whereas if she did not help, she would have one
Will altruism be favored by natural selection?
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
34
Expanding the definition of fitness
Direct fitness: measured by the number of offspring
that an individual has as a result of its own efforts
Indirect fitness: the number of extra offspring that an
individual gains by helping a relative
Devalued by the genetic distance between the
individual and the relative who was helped (in other
words, multiplied by r)
Inclusive fitness: the sum of direct and indirect
fitness
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
35
Discriminating between kin and non-kin
If animals can aid each other
It should be evolutionarily advantageous to
discriminate kin from non-kin
Animals discriminate kin versus non-kin in four ways
Location
Familiarity
Phenotype matching
Recognition alleles
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
36
Kin vs. non-kin: location
Individual animals are often found in predictable
locations
In mammals males generally disperse from home
Females tend to remain in their natal area
Female mammals that help conspecifics located near
their home are likely to be helping relatives
Even if they do not recognize them individually as kin
They follow the rule of thumb “if you are a female, help
those near home” which leads to increased inclusive
fitness
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
37
Location as a mechanism for kin-biased
behavior
When a parent identifies its offspring as those young
in its nest or burrow
Parent birds feed any young in their nest
When the young leave the nest
The parent learns to recognize its young by its calls
And rejects foreign young entering the nest
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
38
A bank swallow feeds any chicks inside its nest,
but ignores its own chicks if they are moved to a
nearby nest hole
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
39
Kin vs. non-kin: familiarity
Young learn to recognize the individuals they are raised
with, through their experiences during early development
Later, they treat familiar and unfamiliar animals differently
A young spiny mouse uses familiarity to distinguish its
siblings
Weanling pups huddle(聚在一起) in pairs
And members of the pair are siblings
Non-kin encountered during the learning process may be
mistakenly classified as kin
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
40
Kin vs. non-kin: phenotype matching
Animals identify kin, even if they have never met
Through physical, behavioral, and physiological
appearances
One’s genetic inheritance has much to do with appearance
Family members often resemble one another
Animals can learn the “kin phenotype”
By learning about the phenotype of familiar individuals
Or by learning their own phenotype
This template is then compared against strangers
“He looks like my brother” or “He looks like me”
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
41
Learning the phenotype of familiar individuals
Developing a template
And then matching new individuals to that template
Belding’s ground squirrels use both familiarity and
phenotype matching
Pups identify their siblings because they learn one
another’s odors while still in the same nest burrow
Juveniles and adults use phenotype matching to
discriminate relatedness among individuals they have
never encountered before
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
42
Paper wasps identify kin from non-kin
By using the odor of hydrocarbons in the insect’s
cuticle (skin)
The odor comes from the nest
When a wasp meets a nestmate away from the nest
It can recognize it
Since a colony consists of a queen and her worker
daughters
The nest odor is a reliable label of colony members as
relatives
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
43
Paper wasps
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
44
An animal can use its own phenotype as
a template
Against which to compare strangers
The “armpit (腋窩) effect:” imagine an individual
sniffing (嗅聞) its own armpit
And then that of a stranger
Golden hamsters discriminate between odors of
unfamiliar kin and unfamiliar non-kin
Using their own odor to form a template
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
45
Kin vs. non-kin: recognition alleles
This mechanism of kin discrimination is genetically based
The allele endows its bearer with a recognizable label
It enables the bearer to perceive that label in others
The bearer behaves preferentially toward others with the
label
This recognition system is called the “green beard effect”
The label could be any conspicuous trait (i.e. a green beard)
As long as the allele responsible for it also causes its owner
to behave appropriately to other labeled individuals
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
46
A green beard allele in the red fire ant
Kin discrimination in fire ants is closely tied to genetics
Memory (formation of a template) also plays a role
The protein-encoding gene, Gp-9, controls social
organization (The gene has two alleles, B and b)
Workers encountering individuals with the b allele form a
template
Workers that come into contact with ants that bear b alleles
when forming their template
Accept only b-bearing queens (bb and Bb)
Workers in colonies with BB individuals accept only BB
queens
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
47
fire ants
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
48
Genetically-based recognition mechanisms
Used to recognize a protein coded by a region of the
DNA
The major histocompatibility complex (MHC)
The MHC region codes for molecules that allow the
body to distinguish between “self” and “nonself”
These genes may serve as direct cues of relatedness
Allowing individuals to identify their kin
Sea squirt (海鞘) larvae use MHC to discriminate kin
Groups of siblings that share an allele in the MHC region
Clump together when they settle on the sea bottom
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
49
Bluebell tunicates (Sea squirt) (海鞘)
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
50
MHC plays a role in kin recognition
Especially in the context of mate choice and incest
avoidance
In amphibians and mammals (including humans)
There is no evidence that MHC genes influence the
perception of odor
Necessary for MHC to be a “recognition allele”
But the MHC system remains an interesting case of a
close link between genes and kin recognition
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
51
Reciprocal altruism
Helping relatives is favored by natural selection
When the inclusive fitness of the helper is increased
Natural selection also favors reciprocal altruism
Altruism between nonrelatives
Evolves if there is an opportunity for payback in the future
“You scratch my back and I’ll scratch yours”
The Prisoner’s Dilemma: in reciprocal altruism
The costs and benefits to the altruist depend on
Whether the recipient returns the favor
Evolutionary game theory can handle situations such as this
The best course of action depends on what others are doing
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
52
The Prisoner’s Dilemma
An imaginary story: two suspects are arrested for a crime
Kept in separate cells to prevent them from communicating
One of them is guilty
But sufficient evidence is lacking for a conviction
The prosecutor offers each a deal
Each prisoner is told that there is enough evidence
To guarantee a short jail term
Freedom can be obtained by providing evidence to send the
other to jail for a long time
If each informs on the other, they both go to jail for an
intermediate length of time
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
53
Constructing a payoff matrix
The strategies available to each player are:
To cooperate (don’t squeal on your partner)
To defect (squeal)
The best strategy: defect while your partner cooperates
Payoff = T (the temptation to defect)
If both you and your partner cooperate the payoff is R
(reward)
If both partners defect, the payoff is P (punishment)
If you cooperate and your partner defects
You get the lowest possible payoff, S (for sucker’s payoff)
The payoffs must be in the order T > R > P > S
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
54
The best strategy for Prisoner’s Dilemma
Examine the payoff matrix
Player 1: the best strategy if the opponent cooperates?
Because T > R, it is better to defect—you will go free
Similarly, if the opponent squeals
Because P > S
It is better to defect, rather than taking the rap
In a single round of the game, it is always better to defect
This is a “dilemma”
If both players follow this logic, they will both defect, and
will both do worse than if they both cooperated
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
55
Tit for tat (一報還一報)
The Prisoner’s Dilemma implies that reciprocal altruism
cannot evolve
If the prisoners never meet again
But individuals do interact, so reciprocal altruism may evolve
The “tit-for-tat” strategy: repeated games of Prisoner’s
Dilemma
An individual begins by being cooperative
In all other interactions: match the other party’s previous
action Be “nice” (begin with cooperation)
Retaliate immediately (if the partner defects, defect in return)
Forgive immediately (“forget” a defection and cooperate if the
partner later cooperates) Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
56
Reciprocal altruism can be an ESS
If a population of individuals adopts this strategy
It cannot be overrun by a selfish mutant that always
defects
When individuals have repeated encounters
Reciprocal altruism can be an evolutionarily stable
strategy (ESS)
Cannot be invaded by another strategy
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
57
When reciprocal altruism is likely to
evolve
Conditions that favor reciprocal altruism:
The benefit of the act to the recipient is greater than the
cost to the actor
The opportunity for repayment is likely to occur
The altruist and the recipient are able to recognize each
other
These factors occur in a highly social species
With a good memory
Long life span
Low dispersal rate
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
58
Reciprocal altruism in vampire bats
Vampire bats bite large mammals and lap up the blood
Consuming up to 50% of their body mass
A bat that did not obtain a blood meal begs for food
A receptive donor regurgitates blood
Regurgitated food sustains the hungry bat until the next
night
When it may find its own meal
Donors give blood to recipients that are not related to
them
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
59
Vampire bats evolved reciprocal altruism
The benefit to the recipient of the blood gift exceeds the
cost to the donor
The recipient gains 12 hours of life and a chance to find food
The donor has two nights of hunting before it would starve
Bats have the opportunity to repay favors and get favors
repaid
Most bats will need blood at some time
They will encounter the same individuals again
Individuals recognize each other
Only bats who have had a prior association share food
Pairs of unrelated bats regurgitated almost exclusively to
each other
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
60
Manipulation
Animals can coerce (迫使) others to help them
Particularly between parents and offspring
Because parents and offspring are not genetically identical
Their interests are not always perfectly aligned
Parents have an advantage in power struggles
They are larger and more experienced than their offspring
Because offspring are related to their parents
Their motivation to resist(反抗) coercion is reduced
If offspring forgo(放棄) their own breeding to help their
parents reproduce
They at least gain indirect fitness
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
61
Cooperation among animals: alarm calls
When Belding’s ground squirrels spot a ground
predator (badgers, snakes, coyotes)
Their alarm call is a series of short sounds
When they spot an aerial predator (hawks and eagles)
Their call is a high-pitched whistle
Are these calls directed at the predator to let it know it
has been detected?
Or are they directed at kin?
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
62
Individual selection in Belding’s ground
squirrels
Individual selection is behind aerial predator alarm calls
When an alarm whistle is heard, pandemonium breaks out
Others also whistle an alarm and all scurry(急趕) to shelter
When a hawk is successful, the victim is usually a noncaller
The most frequent callers were in exposed positions close to
the hawk
The alarm whistles directly benefit the caller
By increasing its chances of escaping predation in the
ensuing chaos (接踵而來的混亂)
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
63
Kin selection in Belding’s ground
squirrels
Individual selection is not behind the squirrels’ alarm trills
Given in response to terrestrial predators
In this case, the caller is assuming a risk
More callers than noncallers are attacked
Predators did not give up when an alarm call was sounded
The caller was not manipulating its neighbors to its
advantage
Other squirrels sat up and looked at the predator
Their reaction did not create chaos that might confuse a
predator
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
64
Female Belding’s ground squirrels are
genetically related
Squirrels practice nepotism (任人唯親): favoritism for
family members
Females are more likely to sound an alarm with a
ground
This is consistent with kinship theory
Females have nearby relatives that benefit from the
warning
Reproductive females are more likely to call
Reproductive females with living relatives call more
frequently
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
65
Expected and observed
frequencies of alarm
calls by Belding’s
ground squirrels in
response to aerial and
terrestrial predators.
Expected frequencies
are those that would be
predicted if the
animals called
randomly. The calls in
response to aerial
predators are close to
the expected
frequencies.
However, the calls in
response to terrestrial
predators are more
likely to be given by
females with relatives
nearby.
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
66
Evolution of alarm calls in rodents
Alarm calling increases the caller’s own chance of survival
Aids the caller’s offspring (i.e. Yellow-bellied marmots)
Or aids other animals
Social species are more likely to call than non-social species
Diurnal (day active) species are more likely to call
Evolution of diurnality precedes evolution of alarm calling
In rodent species, alarm calling communicates with predators
Benefits arising from kin selection are secondary
Belding’s ground squirrels benefit from kin selection
They live in high-density meadows near relatives
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
67
Cooperation in animals: acquiring a mate
Males of some species cooperate in attracting a mate
Some even relinquish (放棄) the opportunity to pass their
alleles into future generations
At least temporarily
These males concentrate their efforts on making another
male more attractive to females
Seems to lower fitness
But in some cases it can be advantageous
Sometimes the benefits to cooperators can arise separately
or in combination with each other
Individual selection, kin selection, or reciprocal altruism
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
68
Cooperation among wild turkeys
Most male wild turkeys in some Texas populations never
mate
A young male and his brothers form a sibling group
Will be an inseparable unit until death
Only the dominant male in each group mates
Each male’s status is decided by the outcome of two contests
Competition for dominance within the sibling group
Endurance is the key to success: the last one able to fight is
the winner
Competition between rival sibling groups
The groups fight until a dominance hierarchy is established
The sibling group with the
most
members
is victorious
Ayo 教材
(動物行為學
2010)
69
Kin selection in wild turkeys
Dominance hierarchies are stable: renegotiation of rank is rare
When the breeding season begins, females visit meadows
The brothers strut (高視闊步) in unison (和諧一致)
Even though only the dominant male in the highest-ranking
sibling group will mate
Kin selection: the major driving force behind this behavior
A subordinate male gains inclusive fitness by helping his brother
perpetuate his alleles
Without his assistance, the brother could not be successful
Siblings must cooperate for their unit to become dominant
Sibling strutting makes the dominant male more attractive
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
70
Cooperation among lions
Male lions cooperate in acquiring
mates
Form coalitions (partnerships) to
challenge males of other prides
The larger coalition usually wins
The reward is a harem of lionesses
Females enter reproductive condition
simultaneously
Any of the males in the coalition may
be the first to mate
A female may change mates
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
Coalitions drive
out resident males
and can involve
fighting
71
Why do male lions form coalitions?
Does kin selection underlie formation of coalitions?
Coalitions usually consist of close relatives
A male gains reproductive success indirectly by helping
his male relatives mate with the female
However, kin selection is not the entire story
Because male coalitions contain unrelated males
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
72
Reciprocal altruism is not important: lions attack a
stuffed lion regardless of the behavior of other lions
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
73
Coalition size affects reproductive success
Male lions in larger coalitions have greater reproductive
success
A solitary male has little chance of reproducing
And much to gain by joining a coalition
By accepting an unrelated male, a small coalition may
take over a pride
The lifetime success of a male lion increases by
cooperating with other males
Even if all the males are not related
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
74
Males in larger coalitions have greater reproductive
success.
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
75
Cooperation among long-tailed manakins
Two or three males of this small bird species work together
They call and perform visual displays to attract a mate
In the up-down jump display: perching males take turns
jumping up and down
In the cartwheel display: one male jumps upward and
backward over the second male
Which then jumps up and over the first male
One male leaves and the remaining male
does a solo performance
The female mates with him
The same male always mates
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
76
Why does a subordinate manakin stay?
It is not indirect fitness
Because the two males are probably not related
A subordinate’ chance of mating is not increased by deserting
Solitary males cannot perform the courtship dance or mate
A subordinate male that outlives his partner obtains a subordinate
partner
Then it will be his turn to mate and raise his direct fitness
Also, singing (which impresses females) improves with age and
practice
The most likely reason for cooperative courtship in this species?
Increased chances of obtaining future direct fitness benefits
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
77
Cooperation among olive baboons
A male olive baboons enlists the help of a friend for a
female
Male A associates (consorts) with an estrous female
Male B (with no female) covets (垂涎) this female
Male B solicits the help of male C to challenge male A
While the battle is in progress, male B mates
Male C risked injury while assisting another to acquire a
mate
Reciprocal altruism: at some time in the future, he will
enlist the help of male B to obtain his own female
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
78
An alliance between two male olive baboons. The
two males on the right are cooperating to challenge
the male on the left. At a later time, the male that
was assisted will have to reciprocate to maintain the
alliance.
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
79
Cooperative breeding
Nonbreeding helpers can reproduce
But stay to help their parents raise young
Acorn woodpeckers fit the definition of cooperative
breeding:
Some individuals (helpers) assist in the care and rearing of
another’s young
Rather than producing offspring of their own
Cooperative breeding it is quite rare
Occurs in birds, mammals, insects and spiders
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
80
Acorn woodpeckers live in family groups
containing nonbreeding helpers
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
81
Helpers provide food
Helpers pitch in to feed offspring
Anybody, not just parents, can collect and carry food
Reduces the parents’ job and increases health and survival
Mammalian mothers provide milk to their offspring
Helpers deliver food by carrying it in their jaws or stomachs
Feeding the mother allows her more time to remain with the
pups instead of hunting
Blackbacked jackals (黑背胡狼)
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
82
Blackbacked jackals (黑背胡狼)
regurgitate food to pups and
mothers
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
83
Helpers protect young and do other jobs
Helpers provide extra protection for the young
Florida scrub jays give alarm calls and mob predators
Jackal families with helpers always have an adult on guard
In fish, helpers protect but don’t feed offspring
In some bird species, helpers may build and clean nests
Or incubate and brood the nestlings
Male helper saddle-backed tamarins (a small primate)
carry offspring
Mothers can obtain nourishment for herself and for an
adequate milk supply
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
84
Is “helping” really helpful?
In many species, the number of helpers correlates with the
survival of the young, the survival of the breeders, or both
Helpers have no effect on the number of eggs laid
But increases the chance that the young hatch, leave the
nest, and become independent birds
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
85
Breeding success for Florida scrub jay pairs with helpers
exceeds that of pairs without helpers
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
86
Do helpers increase breeding success?
Another factor, such as territory quality, may cause an
increase in the number of helpers and breeding success
Removing helpers reduces reproductive success
Gray-crowned babbler nests where helpers were removed
raised fewer young
Superb fairy-wren helpers did not increase offspring
survival
But increased future survival of breeding females
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
87
Meerkat (狐獴) pups gained more weight when the number of
pups was reduced but the number of helpers remained the same
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
88
Do breeders always
want help?
Not always
Helpers might compete for resources
Or increase the chance that the mate has an extra-pair
copulation
In pied kingfishers, helpers are tolerated only when their
services are needed
Primary helpers are older offspring
Secondary helpers, which are unrelated, are permitted to
stay when the additional fish they provide are needed
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
89
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
90
Is it costly to help?
Helping behavior has a cost
Mongoose (貓鼬) helpers forgo feeding and stay at the
burrow to baby-sit and guard pups from predators
While the parents and others forage
They lose weight
Helping may even reduce survival
Helpers among stripe-backed wrens that bring the most
food die more quickly than other birds
If it costly to help, why do it?
(1) Why would an offspring delay dispersal?
(2) Why would it help?
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
91
Mongoose (貓鼬)
stripe-backed wrens
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
92
Stop and think:
Imagine you are spending your graduate school years
studying helping behavior in a little-known bird species
You find that birds that remain behind on their natal
territory to help their parents are far more likely to die
than those that go off to breed on their own
In your dissertation defense, you would like to make the
argument that helping is costly
How confident are you?
What experiment(s) would you like to perform in order to
increase your confidence?
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
93
Why should an offspring delay dispersal?
Animals disperse to avoid inbreeding
Or because of reproductive suppression by their relatives
Or because of competition
Animals exhibit philopatry because they are
Adapted to the local conditions
And familiar with the physical and social settings of home
Dispersal is risky
Small cichlid fish are eaten by predators when they disperse
But, other options may be limited
All available territories might be full
Or all the available mates might be taken
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
94
Delayed dispersal: habitat saturation
Florida scrub jays live in limited scrub habitat
Every territory is filled
A male acquires a territory by
Inheriting a portion of his parents’ property
Replacing his father after his death
Subdivision of his father’s territory
Defeating a breeder
Successfully competing for territory of a breeder that has died
If there is more than one son helping, the dominant one wins
So, scrub jays help their parents because they are making the
best of a bad set of circumstances
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
95
Territory availability and quality
influences helping
When no territories are available, juvenile acorn
woodpeckers
Remain at home and become helpers
Not all territories are equal in quality
It might be better to stay home if the only other
choice is a poor-quality territory
Some Seychelle’s warblers stay to help their
parents rather than move to low-quality territories
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
96
Delayed dispersal: lack of mates
Mates can also be a limited resource
Splendid fairy-wren females suffer much greater annual
mortality than do males
Females are often in short supply
Helpers tend to be males waiting for an available mate
The wait can be as long as five years
When females are scarce, male helpers are plentiful
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
97
Why should a helper help?
Even if an animal does not have the opportunity to breed
That does not automatically mean it will become a helper
Many animals become “floaters” and wander around
without a territory
Even if an individual remains on its own territory
It may not help
There are other reasons beside lack of other
opportunities that underlie helping behavior
Helpers may get indirect fitness benefits
Helpers may get direct fitness benefits
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
98
Helpers get indirect fitness benefits
A helper benefits by increasing the production of relatives
Older offspring help their parents raise siblings
Kin selection can be important
Helping in white-fronted bee-eaters did not provide direct
benefits to the helpers (increased survival or chance of
mating, and increased success in rearing young)
But helping led to increased production of related young
Pied kingfisher become secondary helpers when both their
parents are dead
Long-tailed tit helpers are failed breeders that help relatives
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
99
white-fronted bee-eaters
Pied kingfisher
Long-tailed tit
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
100
Helpers get direct fitness benefits
A helper’s own lifetime reproductive success is increased
by its actions
Many groups have unrelated members
Seychelle’s warblers help raise additional offspring
Extra-pair copulation is common, so a helper may not have
the same father as the offspring it helps
Eggs may be deposited by other birds
Indirect benefits are lower in this species
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
101
On the left is a nest built by a 4-year-old female Seychelles
warbler with no experience.
On the right is a nest built by a 4-year-old female that had
experience in being a helper.
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
102
Direct fitness benefits for helpers
They may add their own eggs to a nest
They may be able to take over a territory budded off
from the main territory
They have a chance to practice their parenting skills
They may mate with the original breeder later on
Or if the original mate dies
In pied kingfishers, secondary helpers (unrelated to the
breeders) mate with the female they had previously
assisted
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
103
Stop and think
Helping behavior in humans has been studied from an
evolutionary perspective
Because human children are very dependent for years
And mothers can give birth to children in rapid succession
Most mothers require help from others
In a review of studies across many cultures
The presence of maternal grandmothers (the mother’s
mother) and sibling helpers improves child survival
Fathers improved child survival in only 1/3 of the studies
What a hypothesis based on kin selection might explain
this?
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
104
Bees are a eusocial (“truly social”) species
Young worker honeybees feed the queen’s larval offspring
Other workers maintain the hive and carry out the dead
Older workers forage at flowers and communicate their location
to their hive mates in an elaborate dance
All workers are female
Only one individual in the hive - a queen - lays eggs
Eusocial species are defined by three characteristics
Reproductive division of labor
Cooperation in the care of young
At least two generations share in the colony’s labor
These characteristics also apply to some cooperative breeders
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
105
A eusociality continuum
How evenly is reproduction is shared among group
members?
At one end, all or many of the group members breed
At the other end, breeding is restricted to one or several
group members
Reproductive skew: describes the degree of eusociality
The proportion of individuals that give up reproduction
Eusociality is rare
Occurs in ants, bees, wasps, termites, aphids, an
ambrosia beetle, thrips, spiders and snapping shrimp
Eusociality is extremely rare in mammals
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
106
Eusociality and cooperative breeding
Predicted locations of cooperatively breeding species along the
eusociality continuum
•
When reproduction is restricted to a single individual
– Reproductive skew = 1
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
107
Naked mole rats are eusocial
Breeding is restricted to a single female - the queen
Other adult females are smaller and don’t ovulate or breed
Colonies contain overlapping generations of offspring
Which are communally cared for
Duties of nonbreeder members depend on their size and
age
Smaller member gather food and transport nest material
Larger members clear the tunnels of obstructions
The largest members dig tunnels and defend the colony
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
108
A queen naked mole rat, the only reproductive female
of the colony, is resting on the workers that feed her
and help care for the young.
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
109
Eusociality presents an obvious puzzle
Members of eusocial species may relinquish all chances
of reproduction
Eusocial colonies can be described as “superorganisms”
Members function efficiently together to ensure survival
and reproductive success of the colony
The most common feature of eusocial societies
They are family groups
What favors the evolution of eusociality?
How do eusocial colonies function on a day-to-day basis?
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
110
Evolutionary origins of eusociality:
haplodiploidy
Haplodiploidy: an unusual genetic system in eusocial
species
The females are diploid, with two sets of chromosomes
The males are haploid, with a single set of chromosomes
Males grow from an unfertilized egg
And produce sperm that are genetically identical to
themselves
When a male mates with a female
The female offspring get a sampling of 50% of their
mother’s DNA
But all of the father’s DNA
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
111
Haploidiploidy changes coefficients of
relatedness in a family
Full sisters (share both a mother and a father) get half of
their DNA from their mothers
They share 50% of their maternally derived DNA
Full sisters also get half their DNA from their fathers
Because fathers are haploid and all sperm are identical
This DNA is identical for every sister
With the paternally and maternally derived DNA
combined
Full sisters share 75% of their DNA with one another (r =
0.75)
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
112
The genetic
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
contributions of
a male and
female of a
haplodiploid
species to their
offspring.
Males are
haploid and
have only one
set of
chromosomes.
They arise from
unfertilized eggs.
A female of a
haplodiploid
species shares
more DNA with
her full sister
113
The relationship between haplodiploidy
and eusociality
Haplodiploidy is not necessary for the evolution of
eusociality
Many eusocial species, such as termites, are not
haplodiploid
Haplodiploidy alone is not sufficient for the evolution of
eusociality
There are many haplodiploid species that are not eusocial
Even in haplodiploid species that are eusocial
Females very often mate with more than one male
Multiple mating reduces the relatedness among sisters
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
114
Summary
Benefits of group living: increased foraging success, decreased
predation risk, conservation of heat, water and energy
Costs: competition, disease, parasites, reproductive interference
Altruism: performance of a service benefiting a conspecific at a
cost to the one that does the deed - it is measured in units of
fitness
Hypotheses for the evolution of altruism:
Individual selection: an individual receives direct benefits
Kin selection: indirect fitness increases by helping kin
Reciprocal altruism: the altruist’s final gain exceeds its cost
Manipulation: animals can coerce others into helping them
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
115
Summary
Relatives are identified through location, familiarity, phenotype
matching, and genetics
Reciprocal altruism occurs when: a recipient’s benefits exceeds
the actor’s costs, future repayment arises, individuals recognize
one another
Cooperation in acquiring a mate: through displays, coalitions or
alliances
Cooperative breeding: altruism where a helper helps rear
offspring not its own
Eusocial species: an extreme example of cooperative breeding
Division of labor, cooperation in caring for young,
generational overlap in colony care
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
116
Summary
Haplodiploid sisters are more related to each other than
to their mothers or daughters
By Hamilton’s Rule, kin selection is more likely to evolve
Factors favoring evolution of eusociality: extended
parental care, long-lasting sibling associations, sharing a
central resource
Eusocial individuals favor colony-mates that are more
closely related
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
117
問題與討論
[email protected]
Ayo 台南 NUTN 站
http://myweb.nutn.edu.tw/~hycheng/
Ayo 教材 (動物行為學 2010)
118