PSY 369: Psycholinguistics - the Department of Psychology at
Download
Report
Transcript PSY 369: Psycholinguistics - the Department of Psychology at
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Language Acquisition
Announcements
Language development section includes information
from Chapter 3, pages 72-87
Homework #2 due Feb 20 (moved from Feb 18)
Homework #1
Two banks in neighboring towns were reported robbed by
the state police yesterday.
/T/ /oo/ /b/ /a/ /n/ /k/ /s/ /i/ /n/ …
main point: not a one to one correspondence between letters and sounds
Free: two, bank, in, neighbor, town, were, report (but not “port”), rob, by, the, state, police,
day
Bound: -s, -ing, -ed, yesterPolice reported the robbery the day before, vs the police robbed the store the day before
Bank as either financial institution or a riverside (or other meanings of bank)
Pragmatics involves how the utterance is used. In Context A it is being used to convey the
news, while it B it is about whether the security needs to be upgraded
Spoken vs. written language. At the “front end” the signals are very different, so the
processing must be different. However, at some point the processes seem to converge. I
was looking for something along the lines of thoughts like these.
Acquiring language
Dr. Cutting, language sure
is complicated. How do you
expect us to learn all this stuff?
Student in my
psycholinguistics
course
Acquiring language
Whadda’ ya mean, mommy.
I can talk.
I can understand what you say.
What’s so hard?
Student in my
psycholinguistics
course
2 year old
Acquiring language
How do we (humans) do it? How do we learn to use
this complex behavior?
Student in my
psycholinguistics
course
2 year old
Overview
Some of the major issues
Imitation vs Innateness
Born to walk
Born to talk?
How much explicit teaching do we get?
Very little on syntax & phonology, some on meaning
Commonalities across individuals, languages and
cultures
Language is complex everywhere
Sounds, words, syntax, and more
No primitive (simple) languages
Language development is similar everywhere
Similar stages
Different approaches
Behaviorist accounts
Imitation (& conditioning) accounts
e.g., B. F. Skinner – children learn through imitation and
reinforcement.
Nativist (Innateness) accounts
o
e.g., Chomsky’s Language Acquisition Device (LAD)
Language works by internalizing the rules of grammar to produce
sentences.
o They do this without practice, reinforcement, or adult modeling
o Universal Grammar & Parameter setting
o
Different approaches
Cognitive hypotheses
Piaget – cognitive development drives language
development
Vygotsky – language and cognition are initially separate, but
as each develop become tightly interconnected, with each
influencing each other
Social hypothesis
e.g., Bruner’s Language acquisition socialization system
(LASS) – emphasized the social setting in acquiring
language
Exposure to language is not enough, learners must
experience language in social/interactive contexts
E.g, child-directed speech, turn taking situations
Typical language development
36 Months
24 Months
18 Months
12 Months
6 Months
Similar stages
Typical language development
6 Months
Responds to his name
Responds to human voices without
visual cues by turning his head and
eyes
Responds appropriately to friendly
and angry tones
Typical language development
12 Months
Uses one or more words with meaning
(this may be a fragment of a word)
Understands simple instructions,
especially if vocal or physical cues are
given
Practices inflection
Is aware of the social value of speech
Typical language development
18 Months
Has vocabulary of approximately 5-20 words
Vocabulary made up chiefly of nouns
Some echolalia (repeating a word or phrase
over and over)
Is able to follow simple commands
Typical language development
24 Months
Can name a number of objects common to his
surroundings
Is able to use at least two prepositions
Combines words into a short sentence (telegraphic)
Vocabulary of approximately 150-300 words
Volume and pitch of voice not yet well-controlled
Typical language development
36 Months
Use pronouns I, you, me correctly
Is using some plurals and past tenses
Knows at least three prepositions
Handles three word sentences easily
Has in the neighborhood of 900-1000 words
About 90% of what child says should be intelligible
Verbs begin to predominate
In the beginning… and the womb
Prelinguistic communication
What was that?
You’re
mumbling.
We experience language before we’re even
born
Normal human language uses sounds between
100 and 4000 Hz
Sound travels through skin and fluids too
In the womb, sounds up to 1000 Hz
Can’t hear individual words
But can hear:
Intonation, durations, rhythm, stress
Add link to Annie Murphy Paul’s TED talk (first 5-6 mins):
http://www.ted.com/talks/annie_murphy_paul_what_we_learn
_before_we_re_born.html
In the beginning… and the womb
Prelinguistic communication
We experience language before we’re even
born
Non-Nutritive Sucking
method
DeCasper & Spence (1986)
Had mothers read stories everyday
to fetuses during final 6 weeks of
pregnancy
After babies were born tested to see
if babies preferred familiar story over
novel one
Results: babies preferred the familiar
stories
In the beginning… and the womb
Prelinguistic communication
We experience language before we’re even
born
Mahler et al. (1988, in France)
4 day old babies
Non-Nutritive Sucking method
Played French or Russian
Sucking pattern changed if
language was switched
Sucking pattern didn’t change if
language wasn’t switched
Babies knew (something about) the
languages (most likely prosody)
In the beginning… and the womb
Prelinguistic communication
We experience language before we’re even
born
Fetal heart monitor
DeCasper, et al (1994)
In the beginning… and the womb
Prelinguistic communication
We experience language before we’re even
born
Fetal heart monitor
DeCasper, et al (1994)
Had mothers read stories everyday
to fetuses during 34-38 weeks of
pregnancy
After 38th week, two stories were
played to the fetuses (but mom
couldn’t hear it)
Same story
Different story
In the beginning… and the womb
Prelinguistic communication
We experience language before we’re even
born
Fetal heart monitor
DeCasper, et al (1994)
Had mothers read stories everyday
to fetuses during 34-38 weeks of
pregnancy
After 38th week, two stories were
played to the fetuses (but mom
couldn’t hear it)
Same story
Different story
In the beginning… and the womb
Prelinguistic communication
We experience language before we’re even
born
Fetal heart monitor
DeCasper, et al (1994)
Had mothers read stories everyday
to fetuses during 34-38 weeks of
pregnancy
After 38th week, two stories were
played to the fetuses (but mom
couldn’t hear it)
Decreased fetal
heart-rate
Same story
Different story
Baby learned something about
the story before it was born!
The early days
Prelinguistic communication
After birth
Child-directed speech (motherese)
Phonological differences are key
Slower
Higher in pitch
More variable in pitch
More exaggerated intonation
All may help to orient and maintain
attention of infant
Typically deal with the “here & now”
May help “bootstrap” later learning
The early days
Prelinguistic communication
After birth
Early “conversations”
Turn taking behaviors
From the movie - breast feeding
“conversations”
Parents interpret infant’s
vocalizations as having meaning
(also from the movie, Snow’s work)
The early days: gestures
Prelinguistic gestures (around 8 months)
Demonstration that the infant is trying to
communicate in some way
e.g., pointing behaviors
Criteria
Waiting
Persistence
Development of alternative plans
The early days: phonology
Eimas et al, (1971)
Categorical perception in infants (1 month olds)
100
Sharp phoneme boundary
Young infants can
distinguish different
phonemes
% /ba/
0
1 ...
3 … 5 …
7
The early days: phonology
Categorical perception in infants
A number of studies suggest that very young
infants can perceive between a number of
phonemic distinctions (e.g., Kuhl & Meltzhoff, 1997)
Not limited to their language context
However, as they age/experience their context
language the ability to perceive some of these
distinctions are lost (~10 to 12 months)
Nature/nurture debate:
Are humans “pre-programmed” to distinguish speech
sounds?
Add link to Patricia Kuhl’s TED talk (~10 mins):
http://www.ted.com/talks/patricia_kuhl_the_linguistic_g
enius_of_babies.html
We’re listening
The early days: phonology
Eimas et al, (1971)
Categorical perception in infants (1 month olds)
100
Sharp phoneme boundary
Chinchillas do it too!
% /ba/
Kuhl and Miller (1975)
0
1 ...
3 … 5 …
7
Are they “preprogrammed to
perceive human
speech?
The early days: speech production
Vocal track differences
Infant
Adult
Infants vocal tracts are smaller, and initially shaped differently
The infant’s tongue fills the entire mouth, reducing the range of
movement
As the facial skeleton grows, the range for movement increases (which
probably contributes to the increased variety of sounds infants start to
produce)
May be (in part) why production lags behind comprehension
Speech production
The progression of cooing and babbling
follows a universal pattern.
Role of both nature and nurture
Pre 6 weeks – “vegetative” sounds
Nature/Biology plays an important role in the
emergence of cooing & babbling.
The form of the child’s vocalization is also
affected by the linguistic environment.
Cry, burp, sucking noises
Post 6 week – “cooing” and later
“babbling”
Babbling & other videos
Speech production
The progression of cooing and babbling
follows a universal pattern.
Babies, until around 6 months old, can produce
sounds/phonemes that their parents cannot
produce or distinguish
6 - 8 weeks: cooing
4 - 6 months: babbling
Clear consonants and vowels are produced
“da”, “gi”
Speech production
The progression of cooing and babbling
follows a universal pattern.
Babies, until around 6 months old, can produce
sounds/phonemes that their parents cannot
produce or distinguish
6 - 8 weeks: cooing
4 - 6 months: babbling
6 - 7 months: Reduplicated babbling
“dada”, “gigi”
Speech production
The progression of cooing and babbling
follows a universal pattern.
Babies, until around 6 months old, can produce
sounds/phonemes that their parents cannot
produce or distinguish
6 - 8 weeks: cooing
4 - 6 months: babbling
6 - 7 months: Reduplicated babbling
8 - 9 months: CVC clusters may appear
“bod”, “tat”
Speech production
The progression of cooing and babbling
follows a universal pattern.
Babies, until around 6 months old, can produce
sounds/phonemes that their parents cannot
produce or distinguish
10 or 11 months: Variegated babbling
Combining “incomprehensible words”
Intonation patterns
“dab gogotah”
May reflect phonological rules of spoken language
context
By 12 to 14 months some evidence of language
specific phonological rules
The first words
Of course he said “arf.”
What else did you expect
his first word to be?
Language Sponges
Learning words
12 ms
2 yrs
3 yrs
6 yrs
first words
200 words
1,000 words
15,000 words
About 3,000 new words per year, especially in the primary
grades
As many as 8 new words per day
Production typically lags behind comprehension
Language Sponges
Lots of individual differences
But there is also a consistent pattern
Vocabulary growth
Methods used to study this
Observational data (60s to present)
Diary studies
Taped language samples (Roger Brown)
Parents record their kids language development
Small numbers of children (Eve, Adam, Sarah)
Went to home every month made tape recordings
Extensive study needed
Hard to kids to “say all the words you know” or “say a
question”
Early phonological production isn’t like adult production,
often need to take great care deciding what the child
meant
Large database CHILDES
Many kids, many languages, including children with language
difficulties
Language Sponges
Learning words
General patterns and observations
Sounds
Meaning
Proposed Strategies
Fast mapping
Whole object
Mutual exclusivity
Learning Syntax
Learning Morphology
Early word learning
First words (Around 10-15 months)
Emergence of systematic, repeated productions of
phonologically consistent forms
1 word stage typically lasts around 10 months
Have learned first 50 words by 15 – 24 months
Typically focused on the “here and now”
Early word learning
First words (Around 10-15 months)
Emergence of systematic, repeated productions of
phonologically consistent forms
Idiomorphs - personalized words
Developed in systematic ways
Not simply imitation, rather are creative
Learned importance of consistency of names
“Adult words” - Typically context bound (relevant to the
immediate environment)
Important people, Objects that move, Objects that can be
acted upon, Familiar actions
Nouns typically appear before verbs
What kinds of words?
1-general names
2- specific names
“red”
5-personal/social
“bye-bye”
4-modifiers
“mommy”
3-action words
“dog”
“yes, no, please”
6-functional
“what”
Early speech production
Transition to speech
This Your
is your
fis?
fis?
Oh, your fish.
No.No.
…My
my fis!
fis.
Yes, my fis.
Early speech production
Transition to speech
Can’t hear the difference?
Your fis.
Oh, your fish.
Can’t produce the correct
sounds?
Rejects adult saying fis
This is your fis? No, … my fis.
Sometimes, but evidence
suggests not always the case
More general process of
simplification
“frees up” resources for
concentrating on other aspects of
language learning
No, my fis.
Yes, my fis.
Early speech production
Transition to speech
individual diffs, but some common processes
Common Phonological processes
Reduction
Delete sounds from words (“da” for dog)
Coalescence
Combine different syllables into one syllable (“paf”
for pacifier)
Assimilation
Change one sound into a similar sound within the
word (“fweet” for sweet)
Reduplication
One syllable from a multi-syllabic word is repeated
(“baba” for bottle)
Extensions of meaning
Applying the words to referents
Extension
Finding the appropriate limits of the meaning of
words
Underextension
Applying a word too narrowly
Overextension
Applying a word too broadly
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
“googie
”
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
1:11,24
“googie
”
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
1:11,24
1:11,25
“tee/hosh”
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
1:11,24
1:11,25
1:11,26
“tee/hosh”
“hosh”
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
1:11,24
1:11,25
1:11,26
1:11,27
“tee/hosh”
“hosh”
“pushi”
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
1:11,24
1:11,25
1:11,26
1:11,27
2:0,10
“tee/hosh”
“hosh”
“pushi”
“mooka”
“hosh”
Extensions of meaning
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
1:11,24
1:11,25
1:11,26
1:11,27
2:0,10
2:0,20
“tee/hosh”
“hosh”
“pushi”
“mooka”
“hosh”
“biggie
One-word-per-referent heuristic
Extensions of meaning
If a new word comes in for a referent that is already named, replace it
Exception to that was “horse,” but it only lasted a day here
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
1:11,24
1:11,25
1:11,26
1:11,27
2:0,10
2:0,20
“tee/hosh”
“hosh”
“pushi”
“mooka”
“hosh”
“biggie
googie”
Strategies for learning
Expansion and contraction can occur at the same time
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
1:11,24
1:11,25
1:11,26
1:11,27
2:0,10
2:0,20
“tee/hosh”
“hosh”
“pushi”
“mooka”
“hosh”
“biggie
googie”
Strategies for learning
Child tries different things, if a word doesn’t work then
try something else
e.g., hosh didn’t for for the large dog, switched to
biggie doggie
“tee”
1:9,11
1:10,18
1:11,1
1:11,2
“googie
”
1:11,24
1:11,25
1:11,26
1:11,27
2:0,10
2:0,20
“tee/hosh”
“hosh”
“pushi”
“mooka”
“hosh”
“biggie
googie”
Indeterminacy: Frog
Frog
Frog?
Green?
Ugly?
Jumping?
Quine’s gavagai problem
The problem of reference:
a word may refer to a number of referents (real
world objects)
a single object or event has many objects, parts
and features that can be referred to
Frog
Frog?
Green?
Ugly?
Jumping?
Learning word meanings
Learning words
Fast mapping (Carey & Bartlett, 1978)
Using the context to guess the meaning of a word
Please give me the chromium tray. Not
the blue one, the chromium one.
All got the olive tray
Several weeks later still had some of the meaning
Constraints on Word Learning
Learning words
Cognitive Constraints (Markman, 1989)
Perhaps children are biased to entertain certain
hypotheses about word meanings over others
These first guesses save them from logical ambiguity
Get them started out on the right track
Object-scope (whole object) constraint
Taxonomic constraint
Mutual exclusivity constraint
Strategies for learning
Object-scope (whole object) constraint
Words refer to whole objects rather than to parts of
objects
Dog
Strategies for learning
Taxonomic constraint
Words refer to categories of similar objects
Taxonomies rather than thematically related obejcts
‘Here is a lux’
‘Show me another lux’
Strategies for learning
But in ‘no-word’ conditions, they would be
shown the first picture
See this? Can you find another one?
Strategies for learning
% Theme / Category
4 and 5 year olds' choice of theme vs. category
Theme
Category
No word condition
Novel word condition
Strategies for learning
Mutual exclusivity constraint (Markam and Watchel 1988)
Each object has one label & different words refer to
separate, non-overlapping categories of objects
An object can have only one label
‘Show me a dax’:
they choose the corkscrew
because it is a less well known object for which they
don’t have a label yet.
Problem with constraints
Most of the constraints proposed apply only to object
names.
There have been cases where children have been
observed violating these constraints
What about verbs? (Nelson 1988)
Using for example the word ‘car’ only to refer to ‘cars moving
on the street from a certain location’ (Bloom 1973)
The mutual exclusivity constraint would prevent
children from learning subordinate and superordinate
information (animal < dog < poodle)
Language explosion continues
The language explosion is not just the result of simple
semantic development; the child is not just adding
more words to his/her vocabulary.
Child is mastering basic syntactic and morphological
processes.
Language explosion continues
Syntax
Mean length of utterance (MLU) in morphemes
Take 100 utterances and count the number of
morphemes per utterance
Daddy coming. Hi, car. Daddy car comed. Two car outside. It
getting dark. Allgone outside. Bye-bye outside.
# morphemes: 3, 2, 4, 3, 4, 2, 2
‘-ing’ and ‘-ed’ separate
morphemes
‘allgone’ treated as a single word
MLU = morphemes/utterances
= 20/7 = 2.86
Language explosion continues
Syntax
Mean length of utterance (MLU) in morphemes
6
5
4
MLU
3
2
1
0
0
20
40
age (months)
60
Language explosion continues
Proto-syntax (??)
Holophrases (around 1-1.5 years)
Single-word utterances may be used to express more than the
meaning usually attributed to that single word by adults
“dog”
might refer to the dog is drinking water
Typically idiosyncratic, but some conventional/common (e.g.,
indicate the existence of an object, request recurrence of object
or event)
Often combined with intonation or gesture
Controversial claim: May reflect a developing sense of syntax,
but not yet knowing how to use it (e.g., see Bloom, 1973)
Language explosion continues
Syntax
Roger Brown (1973) proposed 5 stages
Stage 1: Telegraphic speech (MLU ~ 1.75; around 24 months)
Children begin to combine words into utterances
Limited to a small set of semantic relations (e.g., nomination,
recurrence, attribution, possession [see table 10.3 for examples])
Debate: learning semantic relations or syntactic (position rules)
“baby sleep” agent+action or Noun Verb
Children in telegraphic speech stage are said to leave out the ‘little
words’ and inflections:
e.g. Mummy shoe NOT Mummy’s shoe
Two cat NOT two cats
Language explosion continues
Syntax
Roger Brown (1973) proposed 5 stages
More than two words
Stages 2 through 5
Stage 2 (MLU ~2.25)
begin to modulate meaning using word order (syntax)
Modulations for number, time, aspect
Gradual acquisition of grammatical morphemes (“-ing”, “-s”
Later stages reflect generally more complex use of syntax (e.g.,
questions, negatives)
How do kids learn the syntax?
Innateness accounts
Semantic bootstrapping
Learned accounts
Acquired from the linguistic input from the environment
It is in the stimulus
How do kids learn the syntax?
Innateness account
Pinker (1984, 1989)
Semantic bootstrapping
Child has innate
knowledge of
syntactic categories
Child learns the
and linking rules
meanings of
some content words Child constructs some
semantic representations
Child makes guesses
of simple sentences
about syntactic structure
based on surface form
and semantic meaning
How do kids learn the syntax?
“It is in the stimulus” accounts (e.g. Bates, 1979)
Speech to children is not impoverished (Snow, 1977)
Children learn grammar by mapping semantic roles (agent,
action, patient) onto grammatical categories (subject, verb,
object)
In all languages there are multiple potential cues indicating
semantic/syntactic relations (e.g., word order, case marking)
Similar words occur in similar linguistic contexts
Acoustic information (e.g., prosody) may provide syntactic cues
Children do not need innate knowledge to learn grammar
Acquiring Morphology
Morphology
Typically things like inflections and prepositions start around
MLU of 2.5 (usually in 2 yr olds)
Remember the Wug experiment (Berko-Gleason, 1958)
Acquiring Morphology
Morphology
This person knows how to rick. She did the same thing yesterday.
Yesterday she ________.
Typically children say that she “ricked.”
Acquiring Morphology
Morphology: order of acquisition
Age (yrs)
Morpheme
Example(s)
2
Present progressive
I driving
2
Articles
A dog, the doctor
2
Plural
Balls
2
Uncontractible Copula
He is asleep, am, are
3
Third person singular
He wants an apple
3
Full progressive
Be + ing, I am singing
3
Regular past tense
She walked
Acquiring Morphology
Children sometimes make mistakes.
My teacher holded the baby rabbits.
Yes
She holded the baby rabbits.
No, she holded them loosely.
Did you say your teacher held the baby rabbit?
What did you say she did?
Did you say held them tightly?
Acquiring Morphology
Children sometimes make mistakes.
My teacher holded the baby rabbits.
This is ungrammatical in the adult language
Shows that children are not simply imitating
In this case, what they produce something that is not in their
input.
Acquiring Morphology
Children sometimes make mistakes.
My teacher holded the baby rabbits.
Why do they make errors like these?
In the case at hand, we have what is called overregularization
The verb hold has an irregular past tense form, held
Because this form is used, the regular past tense-- that with ed-- is not found (*hold-ed)
Acquiring Morphology
The case of verb past tense:
Regular verb forms require no stored knowledge of the
past tense form (wug test)
Past tense is accomplished by applying a past tense
rule (e.g., add -ed) to the verb stem
With irregular verbs something must be memorized
Examples:
Horton heared a Who
I finded Renée
The alligator goed kerplunk
Acquiring Morphology
The case of verb past tense:
Stages in the acquisition of irregular inflections
With regular verbs, the default form -ed is used
With irregulars, lists associating the verb with a
particular form of the past tense have to be memorized:
Past tense is -t when attached to leave, keep, etc.
Is -> was
Dig -> dug
Has -> had
Acquiring Morphology
Stages in the acquisition of irregular inflections
time
Step
1
2
3
4
5
Examples
Description
Noun Verb
Adjective
No inflecti o n
Ma n G o
Bad
Adult form
Me n We n t
Wo r s e
Overregularization Ma n s G o e d Badder
Transition
Me n s Wented Worser
Adult form
Men
We n t
Wo r s e
On the face of it, learning these morphological quirks follows a
peculiar pattern:
Early: correct irregular forms are used
Middle: incorrect regular forms are used
Late: correct forms are used again
Memory & Rules
Why do we find this type of pattern?
Memory and rules
The use of overregularized forms starts at around the
same that that the child is beginning to apply the default
-ed rule successfully
Early: All forms-- whether regular or irregular-- are
memorized
Middle: The regular rule is learned, and in some cases
overapplied
Late: Irregulars are used based on memory, regulars use
the rule (the idea is that if the word can provide its own
past tense from memory, then the past tense rule is
blocked)
Memory & Rules
Why do we find this type of pattern?
Memory and rules
Other accounts
Maratsos (2000) – frequency explanation
It is possible to predict which verbs will be subject to
overregularization
The more often an irregular form occurs in the input, the
less likely the child is to use it as an overregularization
This is evidence that some part of overregularization
occurs because of memory failures
Something about irregulars is unpredictable, hence
has to be memorized
What kind of “teaching” do kids get?
If language is learned (and
not innate), how do kids do
it?
What kind of feedback do
they get?
Claim: Positive evidence is
not sufficient for learning a
language.
What kind of “teaching” do kids get?
Are the kids even aware of mistakes?
The children are apparently aware of the fact that their
forms are strange:
Parent: Where’s Mommy?
Child: Mommy goed to the store
Parent: Mommy goed to the store?
Child: NO! Daddy, I say it that way, not you
Positive and negative evidence
What kind of feedback is available for learning?
Positive evidence: Kids hear grammatical
sentences
Negative evidence: information that a given
sentence is ungrammatical
Kids are not told which sentences are ungrammatical
(no negative evidence)
Let’s consider no negative evidence further…
What kind of “teaching” do kids get?
How much Positive Evidence is there?
Estimated 5000 – 7000 utterances a day
Between ¼ and 1/3 are questions
Over 20% are not “full” adult sentences (typically Noun
or prepositional phrases)
Only about 15% have typical English SVO form
Roughly 45% of all maternal utterances began with one
of 17 words (e.g., “what”, “that”, “it”, “you”)
Cameron-Faulkner, et al (2003)
•
So what kids do hear may be somewhat limited.
Negative evidence
Negative evidence could come in various
conceivable forms.
“The sentence Bill a cookie ate is not a sentence in
English, Timmy. No sentence with SOV word order
is.”
Upon hearing Bill a cookie ate, an adult might
Not understand
Look pained
Rephrase the ungrammatical sentence
grammatically
Kids resist instruction…
McNeill (1966)
Child: Nobody don’t like me.
Adult: No, say ‘nobody likes me.’
Child: Nobody don’t like me.
[repeats eight times]
Adult: No, now listen carefully; say ‘nobody likes me.’
Child: Oh! Nobody don’t likes me.
Kids resist instruction…
Cazden (1972) (observation attributed to Jean Berko Gleason)
Child: My teacher holded the baby rabbits and we patted them.
Adult: Did you say your teacher held the baby rabbits?
Child: Yes.
Adult: What did you say she did?
Child: She holded the baby rabbits and we patted them.
Adult: Did you say she held them tightly?
Child: No, she holded them loosely.
So there doesn’t seem to be a lot of explicit negative evidence, and
what there is the kids often resist
Negative evidence via feedback?
Do kids get “implicit” negative evidence?
Do adults understand grammatical sentences and not understand
ungrammatical ones?
Do adults respond positively to grammatical sentences and
negatively to ungrammatical ones?
Negative evidence via feedback?
Brown & Hanlon (1970):
Case study of “Adam” - looked at things that were said to
him by adults, and what he said to them
Adults understood 42% of the grammatical sentences.
Adults understood 47% of the ungrammatical ones.
Adults expressed approval after 45% of the
grammatical sentences.
Adults expressed approval after 45% of the ungrammatical
sentences.
Suggests that there isn’t a lot of good negative evidence.
In a way, it’s moot anyway…
One of the striking things about child language is how few
errors they actually make.
For negative feedback to work, the kids have to make the errors
(so that it can get the negative response).
But they don’t make enough relevant kinds of errors to
determine the complex grammar.
Pinker, Marcus and others, conclude that much of this stuff
must be innate.
But this isn’t the only view. There is an ongoing debate about
whether there are rules, or whether these patterns of behavior
can be learned based on the language evidence that is available
to the kids
Critical (sensitive) periods
Critical (sensitive) periods
Certain behavior is developed more quickly
within a critical period than outside of it. This
period is biologically determined.
Examples:
Imprinting in ducks (Lorenz, ; Hess, 1973)
Ducklings will follow the first moving thing they see
Only happens if they see something moving within the first
few hours (after 32 hours it won’t happen) of hatching
Binocular cells in humans
Cells in visual system that respond only to input from both
eyes.
If these cells don’t get input from both eyes within first year
of life, they don’t develop
Critical (sensitive) periods
Certain behavior is developed more quickly
within a critical period than outside of it. This
period is biologically determined.
Some environmental input is necessary for normal
development, but biology determines when the
organism is responsive to that input.
That “when” is the critical period
Critical period for language
Lenneberg (1967) proposed that there is a critical
period for human language
It assumes that language acquisition must occur
before the end of the critical period
Estimates range from 5 years up to onset of puberty
Evidence for critical period for language
Feral Children
Children raised in the wild or with reduced exposure to
human language
What is the effect of this lack of exposure on language
acquisition?
Two classic cases
Victor, the Wild Boy of Aveyron
Genie
Victor, The Wild Boy of Aveyron
Found in 1800 near the outskirts of Aveyron, France
Estimated to be about 7-years-old
Considered by some to be the first documented case of autism
Neither spoke or responded to speech
Taken to and studied by Dr. Jean-Marc-Gaspard Itard, and
educator of deaf-mute and retarded children
Never learned to speak and his receptive language ability was
limited to a few simple commands.
Described by Itard as “an almost normal boy who could not speak”
Genie
Found in Arcadia, California in 1970, was not
exposed to human language until age 13.5.
Raised in isolation a situation of extreme abuse
Genie could barely walk and could not talk when
found
Dr. Susan Curtiss made great efforts to teach her
language, and she did learn how to talk, but her
grammar never fully developed.
Only capable of producing telegraphic utterances
(e.g. Mike paint or Applesauce buy store)
Used few closed-class morphemes and function
words
Speech sounded like that of a 2-year-old
Genie
By age of 17 (after 4 years of extensive training)
Vocabulary of a 5 year old
Poor syntax (telegraphic speech mostly)
Examples
Mama wash hair in sink
At school scratch face
I want Curtiss play piano
Like go ride yellow school bus
Father take piece wood. Hit. Cry.
What Do These Cases Tell Us?
Suggestive of the position that there is a critical
period for first language learning (in particular for
syntax and phonological development)
If child is not exposed to language during early childhood
(prior to the age of 6 or 7), then the ability to learn syntax will
be impaired while other abilities are less strongly affected
Not uncontroversial: Victor and Genie and children like them
were deprived in many ways other than not being exposed to
language
Genie stopped talking after age 30 and was institutionalized
shortly afterward (Rymer, 1993)
What Do These Cases Tell Us?
Suggestive of the position that there is a critical
period for first language learning (in particular for
syntax and phonological development)
Why?
Nativist explanation (see pg 79 of text)
Maturational explanation: “less is more”
Second language learning
Learning a new language
What if we already know one language, but want to learn
another?
Adults learning another language typically have a persistent
foreign accent – perhaps a critical period for phonology
(Flege & Hillenbrand, 1984)
Adults typically do better initially at learning a new language
compared to kids, but kids typically do better over the long
term (Krashen, Long, & Scarcella, 1982)
Second language learning
Johnson and Newport (1989)
Native Chinese/Korean speakers moving to US
Task: Listen to sentences and judge whether
grammatically correct
Age and Second-language acquisition
mean score on
English grmmar test
R = -.87
Test score
280
270
260
250
240
230
220
210
200
2
Age of arrival
17
R = -.16
native
3 to 7
8 to 10
age of arrival
11 to 16
17 to 39
Test score
17
Age of arrival
40
Second language learning
Johnson and Newport (1989)
Native Chinese/Korean speakers moving to US
Task: Listen to sentences and judge whether
grammatically correct
Concluded that around the age of 16 something
happens
Different factors operate on language acquisition before
and after the age of 16
Birdsong and Molis (2001)
Replicated the Johnson and Newport study in
Spanish/English speakers.
Did not find a discontinuity around the age of 16
Effects of the Critical Period
Learning a language:
Under 7 years: perfect command of the language possible
Ages 8- c.15: Perfect command less possible progressively
Age 15-: Imperfect command possible
But these claims are far from universally accepted
Bilinguals & Polyglots
Many people speak more than one language
Tucker (1999) - multilinguals outnumber monolinguals
What is the impact of knowing/using more than one
language?
Factors affecting second language acquisition?
What does the lexicon look like?
Interesting effects in bilinguals
Interference
Code switching
Cognitive advantages
Second language acquisition
Contexts of childhood bilingualism
Simultaneous
Both languages are acquired at the same time
Vocabulary growth of bilinguals is similar to that of monolinguals
Some aspects of acquisition may be slowed, but by age of 4
typically caught up
Doesn’t seem to matter whether languages are “related” or not
(e.g., English - French versus English Japanese)
Can achieve “fluency” in both languages
Sequential acquisition
The second language is learned after a first language
When the second language (L2) is acquired is important
Early versus late learning (e.g., see the Johnson and
Newport study)
Second language acquisition
Frequency of usage of both languages
Mode of acquisition
How often and in what contexts do you use the two languages
“Use it or lose it” - language attrition
Native bilingualism - growing up in a two language environment
Immersion - schooling provided in a non-native language
Submersion - one learner surrounded by non-native speakers
Language dominance effects
Relative fluency of L1 and L2 may impact processing
Bilingual Representations
How do we represent linguistic information in a
bilingual lexicon?
Probably depends on many of the factors just discussed
Let’s look at some models and research focusing on the
situation where L1 is dominant relative to L2
Models of the bilingual lexicons
Potter et al (1984): Separate Stores Models – separate
lexicons for each language
Word Association Model
Concept Mediation Model
L1=First Language
L2=Second Language
CONCEPTS
L1
L2
CONCEPTS
L1
L2
Models of the bilingual lexicons
Paivio, Clark, & Lambert (1988): Common Stores
Models – words from both languages in same store
L1=First Language
L2=Second Language
CONCEPTS
L1 & L2
Revised Hierarchical Model
The results are mixed,
supporting more complex
models
May be different in different bilinguals
depending on things like age of acquisition,
relative proficiency, etc.
Kroll & Stewart (1994)
Proposed that the fluency of
L2 needs to be considered in
the processing model
concepts
conceptual
links
conceptual
links
lexical
links
L1
L2
Interesting effects in bilinguals
Interference
Code switching
Cognitive advantages
Interesting effects in bilinguals
Interference
Does knowing two languages lead to interference?
When found, interference is at multiple levels
Phonological - least amount of interference
Lexical - mixing words from different languages
Initially, appear to use a one word per thing strategy
But as they realize there that they’re speaking two language,
then they’ll use words from both languages simultaneously
Syntactic
Until year two, may use only one syntactic system which is
common to both languages
Then a brief period with two sets of lexical items, but still a
common syntax
Finally, two lexicons and two sets of syntax
Interesting effects in bilinguals
Determine who or what is the one performing the action.
The waitress pushes the cowboys.
The telephones pushes the cowboys.
Kisses the table the apple.
The baskets the teacher kicks.
As a native speaker of English we can use many
cues:
Word order
Animacy
Verb agreement
Not all languages use the same cues to the same
extent
e.g., German doesn’t rely as much on word order, but
relies more on agreement processes
Interesting effects in bilinguals
Determine who or what is the one performing the action.
The waitress pushes the cowboys.
The telephones pushes the cowboys.
Kisses the table the apple.
The baskets the teacher kicks.
Kilborn (1989, 1994)
Found that bilinguals (English as second language)
typically carry over the dominant processing strategies
from their native languages.
This interacts with their level of fluency in the second
language
Code switching
When bilinguals substitute a word or phrase from one
language with a phrase or word from another language
“I want a motorcycle VERDE”
Switching is systematic, not random
Code switching
When bilinguals substitute a word or phrase from one
language with a phrase or word from another language
“I want a motorcycle VERDE”
The Spanish adjective “verde” follows a grammatical rule that is
observed by most bilingual speakers that code-switch
“I want a VERDE motorcycle”
Would be incorrect
because language switching can occur only if the adjective is placed
according to the rules of the language of the adjective
In this case, the adjective is in Spanish; therefore, the adjective must
follow the Spanish grammatical rule that states that the noun must
precede the adjective
Code switching
When bilinguals substitute a word or phrase from one
language with a phrase or word from another language
“I want a motorcycle VERDE”
Generally, bilinguals take longer to read and comprehend
sentences containing code-switched words
May be due to a “mental switch mechanism” that determines which
of the bilingual’s two mental dictionaries are “on” or “off” during
language comprehension.
This mental switch is responsible for selecting the appropriate mental
dictionary to be employed during the comprehension of a sentence.
E.g., if reading an English, a Spanish code-switched word is
encountered, the mental switch must disable the English linguistic
system, and enable the Spanish linguistic system.
Code switching
When bilinguals substitute a word or phrase from one
language with a phrase or word from another language
“I want a motorcycle VERDE”
Generally, bilinguals take longer to read and comprehend
sentences containing code-switched words
This time difference depends on similarity of the languages
Chinese-English bilinguals take longer to recognize English codeswitched words in Chinese sentences only if the English words contain
initial consonant-consonant (e.g., flight) clusters, simply because the
Chinese language lacks this phonotactic structure.
Another current view suggests that language dominance (i.e.,
which language is used more frequently) plays an important role in
code-switching
Cognitive advantages
Some evidence suggest that being bilingual can have
an impact on cognition outside of language
Bialystok and colleagues
Bilinguals are very proficient at switching between languages
Bilinguals also have to be good at suppressing the contextually
inappropriate language