Lecture 3 - Framework and Concepts
Download
Report
Transcript Lecture 3 - Framework and Concepts
LING / ASIA 122 - 5
FRAMEWORK AND RELEVANT
CONCEPTS
Based on Kachru and Smith, Chapter 1
and
G. Tucker Childs, in The 5-Minute Linguist
ROAD MAP
Now that we’ve:
defined and identified
global languages,
briefly outlined the
history of English,
described the factors
that led to the
prominence of English
as a global language,
and
seen several varieties
of English from around
the world , . . .
ROAD MAP (cont’d)
And after having taken a
side trip to explore our
linguistic heritages, . . .
In this week’s readings and
lectures, we will . . .
Review some facts about languages and
dialects, and
Explore some concepts that will help us
better analyze what contributes to successful
and less successful communication across
cultures,
Even when everyone is speaking the same
language, i.e., ENGLISH
Languages and Dialects:
Some things to remember
Everyone who speaks a language speaks a dialect;
A language can be seen as a group of dialects;
Dialects can be geographic – Texan, Boston, SoCal
Dialects can be social – “My Fair Lady”
Dialects can be political – “… an army and a navy”
Dialect differences are usually minor
Pronunciation
Grammar
Vocabulary
“Language” and “dialect” are loaded terms
Critical Concepts Forming the
Basis for This Course
Types of Information: conceptual, indexical,
interactional-management
Speech Acts, Implicature, the Cooperative
Principle
Conversation Analysis: Turns, Exchanges,
Turn Relevance Places, Adjacency Pairs
Politeness and Positive and Negative Face
Context: Setting, Participants, Ends, Acts,
Key, Instrumentalities, Norms, Genres
Types of Information
Conceptual information: purely factual
content of linguistic signals.
Indexical information: information about the
speaker / writer.
Interactional-management information:
information that allows participants to
initiate, participate in and terminate
interactions.
Interactional-Management
Information:
What we know about day-to-day
interactions:
◦ How to open conversations
◦ How to hold the floor
◦ When and how to take & yield the floor
◦ How to stay on topic and to change
topics
◦ How to close a conversation
Conversation Analysis – Tools to
analyze interactional-management
information
Conversational floor – the ‘shared space’ in which a
conversation takes place; participants in a
conversation share the conversational floor.
Turn: the distribution of talk across participants; the
stretch of speech of a single speaker bounded by the
speech of another speaker.
Turn1: - A : How did you like Avatar?
Turn 2: - B: I thought it was great!
Conversation Analysis
(cont’d)
Exchange: Two or more sequential turns. For example,
Exchange 1:
A: Could you put this letter in the mailbox for my on your way out?
B: Sure.
Exchange 2:
A: Could I ask you a favor?
B: Sure, what is it?
A: Could you put this letter in the
mailbox for me on your way out?
B: Sure.
Conversation Analysis (cont’d)
Adjacency pair: Two successive utterances or turns
by different speakers, where the second is of a
type required or expected by the first.
In the previous example, the exchange between A and
B constitutes an adjacency pair (question and answer)
Greeting -> Greeting
Apology -> Minimalization
Thanks -> Acknowledgement
Etc.
Conversation Analysis (cont’d)
Turn relevance point (TRP) – the potential
boundary that marks where a turn could end,
marked by one or more of the following:
Phrase final intonation
Grammar
Eye contact
Body movement
Etc.
Conversation Analysis (cont’d)
Repair – the conversational work required when a
conversationalist fails to respond with the expected
turn type
Example: when a speaker
fails to respond to a question
with an answer
Overlaps – occasions when a second speaker begins
before the first speaker has finished
Back-channeling – vocalizations by the listener
relinquishing the floor to the current speaker
‘uh-huh’
‘yeah’
Etc.
Indexical Information:
Presenting One’s Self
Politeness Principle (Lakoff 1973)
◦ Don’t impose.
Pardon me.
I hope I’m not bothering you, but ….
◦ Give options.
Would you mind …?
Could you possibly …?
May I ask you to …?
◦ Make your receiver feel good.
That color really looks good on you.
I like your new tattoo.
“Little white lies”
Indexical information (cont’d)
“Face” (Brown & Levinson 1978): the public self image
that every adult tries to project.
Positive face: the desire of every person to be desirable
to at least some others; the positive consistent selfimage or 'personality' claimed by interactants.
Negative face: the desire of every person to have
his/her actions be unimpeded by others; the basic
claim to territories, personal preserves, rights to nondistraction—i.e., the freedom of action and freedom
from imposition.
Politeness
and Face
The specific nature of face varies from society to
society, e.g.:
Roles of parents and adult children
Notions of privacy in the home, workspace
The precise way of indicating
respect for face may be culture
specific, e.g.:
Offer of a drink and initial
refusal
Refusal of an invitation
Etc.
Speech Acts
The acts we perform when uttering
sentences
1955 – John Austin’s William James Lectures
at Harvard (How to Do Things With Words,
1962)
◦ An utterance can constitute an act:
I promise I’ll be there on time.
I apologize for the way I acted.
Austin’s Criteria for Speech Acts
The sentence must contain a Performative Verb – a
verb that under specified conditions when
uttered, constitutes the performance of an act,
e.g.,
‘I promise you that I won’t be late.’
Must be in the present tense
◦ *I promised that I wouldn’t be late.
Must have a first person subject
◦ *He promises that he won’t be late.
“I hereby” test
◦ I hereby promise that I won’t be late.
Felicity conditions – conditions that
must be met for the utterance to
constitute a valid act
The person and circumstances must be
appropriate:
“I now pronounce you husband and wife.”
The act must be executed completely and correctly
be all participant:
“I bet you SJSU will beat Hawai’i.”
The participants must have the appropriate
intentions:
“I congratulate you for your good fortune.”
Expanding the Notion of Speech Acts
1969 – John Searle’s Speech Acts: all utterances,,
not just those containing performative verbs,
constitute speech acts, thus distinguishing
between:
Explicit Speech Acts:
“I hereby christen thee ‘The Good Ship Lollipop.’”
“I sentence you to life in prison.”
Implicit Speech Acts:
(I declare that) “I ran into Bill and Tony at the movies
last night.”
(I ask) “What time did you get in?”
( request that you) “Please pass the hot sauce.”
Direct and Indirect Speech Acts
Direct: an utterance whose linguistic form matches its
communicative purpose, e.g.,
◦ Form = statement, Purpose = declaration
‘An amoeba is a one-celled animal.’
◦ Form= interrogative, Purpose = question
‘What’s your name?’
◦ Form = imperative, Purpose = order
‘Turn on the lights.
Indirect: an utterance whose linguistic form does not
match it communicative purpose, e.g.,
◦ Form = question, intent = request
‘Is that the phone?’
◦ Form = statement, intent = question
‘I wonder why you would say such a thing.’
How we understand indirect speech acts:
Conversational Implicature
To interpret indirect speech acts, we rely on
implicature, our ability to understand the
speaker’s intention in uttering something e.g.,
A: Is that the phone?
Implicature: A wants me to answer
the phone.
B: I’m in the bathroom.
Implicature: B wants me to know
that B can’t answer the phone.
How we make implicatures:
The Cooperative Principle
Purpose: To describe in a systematic
and consistent way how implicature
works in conversation (H. P. Grice)
“In conversations,
participants cooperate
with each other.”
(Wow!
What does this mean????)
Operationalizing the Cooperative Principle:
Conversational Maxims
Quantity – contribution should be as informative as
required
Quality – contribution should not be false
Relation – contribution should be relevant
Manner – contribution should be direct
Assumptions
1) We don’t adhere to them strictly.
2) We interpret what we hear as if it conforms to them.
3) Where a maxim is violated, we draw implicatures.
Violations of Maxims:
Quantity
◦ Letter of reference:
“Bob speaks perfect English; he doesn’t smoke
in the office; and I have never heard him use
foul language.”
Quality
◦ “Reno is the capital of Nevada, isn’t it?”
◦ “Yes, and London is the capital of New Jersey”
Violations of Maxims:
Relation
“What time is it?”
“Well, the paper’s already come.”
Manner
“Let’s stop and get something to eat.”
“OK, but not at M-c-D-o-n-a-l-d-s.”
Violations of the Maxims
"Uncle Charlie is coming over for dinner."
"Better lock up the liquor.“
"Do you know where Kendall moved?"
"Somewhere on the east coast.“
"How was your blind date?"
"He had a nice pair of shoes.“
“Spencer is sure he'll get that job."
“Yeah. And my pet turtle is sure it will win the
Kentucky Derby."
Context
Observation: The forms that social interactions take and
the meanings they embody are dependent on the context
in which they are uttered:
◦
◦
◦
◦
◦
◦
Setting: Where does the interaction take place?
Participants: Who’s involved?
Ends / goals: What’s the purpose of the interaction?
Acts: What speech acts are employed?
Key: What’s the mood / tenor of the interaction?
Instrumentalities: What modes of interaction are
employed (e.g., phone, text message, face-to face,
etc.)
◦ Norms: What is the norm in this culture for this type of
interaction?
◦ Genre: What kinds of genres are found in this type of
interaction
Activity
Kachru and Smith, Pages 28-29