Faceted Metadata for Information Architecture and Search
Download
Report
Transcript Faceted Metadata for Information Architecture and Search
Unambiguous + Unlimited = Unsupervised
or
Using the Web for
Natural Language Processing Problems
Marti Hearst
School of Information, UC Berkeley
This research supported in part by NSF DBI-0317510
Natural Language Processing
The ultimate goal: write programs that read and
understand stories and conversations.
This is too hard! Instead we tackle sub-problems.
There have been notable successes lately:
Machine translation is vastly improved
Decent speech recognition in limited circumstances
Text categorization works with some accuracy
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Automatic Help Desk Translation at MS
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Why is text analysis difficult?
One reason: enormous vocabulary size.
The average English speaker’s vocabulary is
around 50,000 words,
Many of these can be combined with many
others,
And they mean different things when they do!
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
How can a machine understand these?
Decorate the cake with the frosting.
Decorate the cake with the kids.
Throw out the cake with the frosting.
Get the sock from the cat with the gloves.
Get the glove from the cat with the socks.
It’s in the plastic water bottle.
It’s in the plastic bag dispenser.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
How to tackle this problem?
The field was stuck for quite some time.
CYC: hand-enter all semantic concepts and relations
A new approach started around 1990
How to do it:
Get large text collections
Compute statistics over the words in those collections
Many different algorithms for doing this.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Size Matters
Recent realization: bigger is better than smarter!
Banko and Brill ’01: “Scaling to Very, Very Large
Corpora for Natural Language Disambiguation”, ACL
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Example Problem
Grammar checker example:
Which word to use?
<principal> <principle>
Solution: look at which words surround each use:
I am in my third year as the principal of Anamosa High
School.
School-principal transfers caused some upset.
This is a simple formulation of the quantum mechanical
uncertainty principle.
Power without principle is barren, but principle without
power is futile. (Tony Blair)
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Using Very, Very Large Corpora
Keep track of which words are the neighbors of each
spelling in well-edited text, e.g.:
Principal: “high school”
Principle: “rule”
At grammar-check time, choose the spelling best
predicted by the surrounding words.
Surprising results:
Log-linear improvement even to a billion words!
Getting more data is better than fine-tuning algorithms!
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
The Effects of LARGE Datasets
From Banko & Brill ‘01
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
How to Extend this Idea?
This is an exciting result …
BUT relies on having huge amounts of text
that has been appropriately annotated!
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
How to Avoid Labeling?
“Web as a baseline” (Lapata & Keller 04,05)
Main idea: apply web-determined counts to
every problem imaginable.
Example: for t in {<principal> <principle>}
Compute f(w1, t, w2)
The largest count wins
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web as a Baseline
Works very well in some cases
machine translation candidate selection
article generation
noun compound interpretation
noun compound bracketing
adjective ordering
Significantly better than the
best supervised algorithm.
Not significantly different
from the best supervised.
But lacking in others
spelling correction
countability detection
prepositional phrase attachment
How to push this idea further?
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Using Unambiguous Cases
The trick: look for unambiguous cases to start
Use these to improve the results beyond what cooccurrence statistics indicate.
An Early Example:
Hindle and Rooth, “Structural Ambiguity and Lexical
Relations”, ACL ’90, Comp Ling’93
Problem: Prepositional Phrase attachment
I eat/v spaghetti/n1 with/p a fork/n2.
I eat/v spaghetti/n1 with/p sauce/n2.
quadruple: (v, n1, p, n2)
Question: does n2 attach to v or to n1?
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Using Unambiguous Cases
How to do this with unlabeled data?
First try:
Parse some text into phrase structure
Then compute certain co-occurrences
f(v, n1, p) f(n1, p)
f(v, n1)
Problem: results not accurate enough
The trick: look for unambiguous cases:
Spaghetti with sauce is delicious. (pre-verbal)
I eat it with a fork. (object of preposition can’t attach
to a pronoun)
Use these to improve the results beyond what cooccurrence statistics indicate.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Unambiguous + Unlimited = Unsupervised
Apply the Unambiguous Case Idea to the Very, Very Large
Corpora idea
The potential of these approaches are not fully realized
Our work:
Structural Ambiguity Decisions (work with Preslav Nakov)
PP-attachment
Noun compound bracketing
Coordination grouping
Semantic Relation Acquisition
Hypernym (ISA) relations
Verbal relations between nouns
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Structural Ambiguity Problems
Apply the U + U = U idea to structural ambiguity
Noun compound bracketing
Prepositional Phrase attachment
Noun Phrase coordination
Motivation: BioText project
In eukaryotes, the key to transcriptional regulation of the Heat Shock
Response is the Heat Shock Transcription Factor (HSF).
Open-labeled long-term study of the subcutaneous sumatriptan efficacy
and tolerability in acute migraine treatment.
•
BimL protein interact with Bcl-2 or Bcl-XL, or Bcl-w proteins (Immunoprecipitation (anti-Bcl-2 OR Bcl-XL or Bcl-w)) followed by Western blot
(anti-EEtag) using extracts human 293T cells co-transfected with EEtagged BimL and (bcl-2 or bcl-XL or bcl-w) plasmids)
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Applying U + U = U to Structural Ambiguity
We introduce the use of (nearly) unambiguous
features:
surface features
paraphrases
Combined with very, very large corpora
Achieve state-of-the-art results without labeled
examples.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Noun Compound Bracketing
(a)
(b)
[ [ liver cell ] antibody ]
[ liver [cell line] ]
(left bracketing)
(right bracketing)
In (a), the antibody targets the liver cell.
In (b), the cell line is derived from the liver.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Dependency Model
right bracketing: [w1[w2w3] ]
w2w3 is a compound (modified by w1)
home health care
w1 and w2 independently modify w3
adult male rat
w1
w2
w3
w1
w2
w3
left bracketing : [ [w1w2 ]w3]
only 1 modificational choice possible
law enforcement officer
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Related Work
Marcus(1980), Pustejosky&al.(1993), Resnik(1993)
adjacency model:
Lauer (1995)
Pr(w1|w2) vs. Pr(w2|w3)
dependency model: Pr(w1|w2) vs. Pr(w1|w3)
Keller & Lapata (2004):
use the Web
unigrams and bigrams
Our approach:
Girju & al. (2005)
• Web as data
supervised model
2 , n-grams
•
bracketing in context
• paraphrases
requires WordNet senses
• surface features
to be given
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Computing Bigram Statistics
Dependency Model, Frequencies
Compare #(w1,w2) to #(w1,w3)
Dependency model, Probabilities
Pr(left) = Pr(w1w2|w2)Pr(w2w3|w3)
Pr(right) = Pr(w1w3|w3)Pr(w2w3|w3)
right
w1
w2
left
So we compare Pr(w1w2|w2) to Pr(w1w3|w3)
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
w3
Probabilities: Estimation
Using page hits as a proxy for n-gram counts
Pr(w1w2|w2) = #(w1,w2) / #(w2)
#(w2)
#(w1,w2)
word frequency; query for “w2”
bigram frequency; query for “w1 w2”
smoothed by 0.5
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Association Models: 2 (Chi Squared)
A = #(wi,wj)
B = #(wi) – #(wi,wj)
C = #(wj) – #(wi,wj)
D = N – (A+B+C)
N = 8 trillion (= A+B+C+D)
8 billion Web pages x 1,000 words
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features
Authors often disambiguate noun compounds using
surface markers, e.g.:
amino-acid sequence left
brain stem’s cell left
brain’s stem cell right
The enormous size of the Web makes these
frequent enough to be useful.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features:
Dash (hyphen)
Left dash
cell-cycle analysis left
Right dash
donor T-cell right
fiber optics-system should be left..
Double dash
T-cell-depletion unusable…
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features:
Possessive Marker
Attached to the first word
brain’s stem cell right
Attached to the second word
brain stem’s cell left
Combined features
brain’s stem-cell right
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features:
Capitalization
don’t-care – lowercase – uppercase
Plasmodium vivax Malaria left
plasmodium vivax Malaria left
lowercase – uppercase – don’t-care
brain Stem cell right
brain Stem Cell right
Disable this on:
Roman digits
Single-letter words: e.g. vitamin D deficiency
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features:
Embedded Slash
Left embedded slash
leukemia/lymphoma cell right
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features:
Parentheses
Single-word
growth factor (beta) left
(brain) stem cell right
Two-word
(growth factor) beta left
brain (stem cell) right
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features:
Comma, dot, semi-colon
Following the first word
home. health care right
adult, male rat right
Following the second word
health care, provider left
lung cancer: patients left
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features:
Dash to External Word
External word to the left
mouse-brain stem cell right
External word to the right
tumor necrosis factor-alpha left
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Web-derived Surface Features:
Problems & Solutions
Problem: search engines ignore punctuation in
queries
“brain-stem cell” does not work
Solution:
query for “brain stem cell”
obtain 1,000 document summaries
scan for the features in these summaries
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Other Web-derived Features:
Abbreviation
After the second word
tumor necrosis factor (NF) right
After the third word
tumor necrosis (TN) factor right
We query for, e.g., “tumor necrosis tn factor”
Problems:
Roman digits: IV, VI
States: CA
Short words: me
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Other Web-derived Features:
Concatenation
Consider health care reform
healthcare : 79,500,000
carereform : 269
healthreform: 812
Adjacency model
healthcare vs. carereform
Dependency model
healthcare vs. healthreform
Triples
“healthcare reform” vs. “health carereform”
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Other Web-derived Features:
Reorder
Reorders for “health care reform”
“care reform health” right
“reform health care” left
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Other Web-derived Features:
Internal Inflection Variability
Vary inflection of second word
tyrosine kinase activation
tyrosine kinases activation
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Other Web-derived Features:
Switch The First Two Words
Predict right, if we can reorder
adult male rat
male adult rat
as
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrases
The semantics of a noun compound is often made
overt by a paraphrase (Warren,1978)
Prepositional
stem cells in the brain right
cells from the brain stem right
Verbal
virus causing human immunodeficiency left
Copula
office building that is a skyscraper right
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrases
prepositional paraphrases:
We use: ~150 prepositions
verbal paraphrases:
We use: associated with, caused by, contained in, derived
from, focusing on, found in, involved in, located at/in,
made of, performed by, preventing, related to and used
by/in/for.
copula paraphrases:
We use: is/was and that/which/who
optional elements:
articles: a, an, the
quantifiers: some, every, etc.
pronouns: this, these, etc.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Evaluation: Datasets
Lauer Set
244 noun compounds (NCs)
from Grolier’s encyclopedia
inter-annotator agreement: 81.5%
Biomedical Set
430 NCs
from MEDLINE
inter-annotator agreement: 88% ( =.606)
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Evaluation: Experiments
Exact phrase queries
Limited to English
Inflections:
Lauer Set: Carroll’s morphological tools
Biomedical Set: UMLS Specialist Lexicon
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Co-occurrence Statistics
Lauer set
Bio set
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrase and Surface Features Performance
Lauer Set
Biomedical Set
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Individual Surface Features Performance: Bio
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Individual Surface Features Performance: Bio
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Results Lauer
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Results: Comparing with Others
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Results Bio
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Summary:
Results for Noun Compound Bracketing
Introduced search engine statistics that go
beyond the n-gram (applicable to other
tasks)
surface features
paraphrases
Obtained new state-of-the-art results on NC
bracketing
more robust than Lauer (1995)
more accurate than Keller&Lapata (2004)
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Prepositional Phrase Attachment
(a) Peter spent millions of dollars.
(b) Peter spent time with his family.
(noun attach)
(verb attach)
quadruple: (v, n1, p, n2)
(a) (spent, millions, of, dollars)
(b) (spent, time, with, family)
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Noun Phrase Coordination
(Modified) real sentence:
The Department of Chronic Diseases and Health
Promotion leads and strengthens global efforts to
prevent and control chronic diseases or disabilities
and to promote health and quality of life.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
NC coordination: ellipsis
Ellipsis
car and truck production
means car production and truck production
No ellipsis
president and chief executive
All-way coordination
Securities and Exchange Commission
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Results
428 examples from Penn TB
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Semantic Relation Detection
Goal: automatically augment a lexical database
Many potential relation types:
ISA (hypernymy/hyponymy)
Part-Of (meronymy)
Idea: find unambiguous contexts which (nearly)
always indicate the relation of interest
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Lexico-Syntactic Patterns
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Lexico-Syntactic Patterns
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Adding a New Relation
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Semantic Relation Detection
Lexico-syntactic Patterns:
Should occur frequently in text
Should (nearly) always suggest the relation of interest
Should be recognizable with little pre-encoded
knowledge.
These patterns have been used extensively by
other researchers.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Semantic Relation Detection
What relationship holds between two nouns?
olive oil – oil comes from olives
machine oil – oil used on machines
Assigning the meaning relations between these
terms has been seen as a very difficult solution
Our solution:
Use clever queries against the web to figure out the
relations.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Queries for Semantic Relations
Convert the noun-noun compound into a query of the form:
noun2 that * noun1
“oil that * olive(s)”
This returns search result snippets containing interesting
verbs.
In this case:
Come from
Be obtained from
Be extracted from
Made from
…
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Queries for Semantic Relations
More examples:
Migraine drug -> treat, be used for, reduce, prevent
Wrinkle drug -> treat, be used for, reduce, smooth
Printer tray -> hold, come with, be folded, fit under,
be inserted into
Student protest -> be led by, be sponsored by, pit, be,
be organized by
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Conclusions
The enormous size of the web opens new
opportunities for text analysis
There are many words, but they are more likely to
appear together in a huge dataset
This allows us to do word-specific analysis
Unambiguous + Unlimited = Unsupervised
We’ve applied it to structural and semantic language
problems.
These are stepping stones towards sophisticated
language understanding.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Thank you!
http://biotext.berkeley.edu
Supported in part by NSF DBI-0317510
Using n-grams to make predictions
Say trying to distinguish:
[home health] care
home [health care]
Main idea: compare these co-occurrence
probabilities
“home health” vs
“health care”
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Using n-grams to make predictions
Use search engines page hits as a proxy for n-gram
counts
compare Pr(w1w2|w2) to Pr(w1w3|w3)
Pr(w1 w2|w2 ) = #(w1,w2) / #(w2)
#(w2)
#(w1,w2)
word frequency; query for “w2”
bigram frequency; query for “w1 w2”
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Probabilities: Why? (1)
Why should we use:
(a) Pr(w1w2|w2), rather than
(b) Pr(w2w1|w1)?
Keller&Lapata (2004) calculate:
AltaVista queries:
(a): 70.49%
(b): 68.85%
British National Corpus:
(a): 63.11%
(b): 65.57%
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Probabilities: Why? (2)
Why should we use:
(a) Pr(w1w2|w2), rather than
(b) Pr(w2w1|w1)?
Maybe to introduce a bracketing prior.
Just like Lauer (1995) did.
But otherwise, no reason to prefer either one.
Do we need probabilities? (association is OK)
Do we need a directed model? (symmetry is OK)
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Adjacency & Dependency (2)
right bracketing: [w1[w2w3] ]
w2w3 is a compound (modified by w1)
w1 and w2 independently modify w3
adjacency model
Is w2w3 a compound?
(vs. w1w2 being a compound)
w1
w2
w3
w1
w2
w3
w1
w2
w3
dependency model
Does w1 modify w3?
(vs. w1 modifying w2)
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrases: pattern (1)
(1)v n1 p n2 v n2 n1
Can we turn “n1 p n2” into a noun compound “n2 n1”?
meet/v demands/n1 from/p customers/n2
meet/v the customer/n2 demands/n1
Problem: ditransitive verbs like give
(noun)
gave/v an apple/n1 to/p him/n2
gave/v him/n2 an apple/n1
Solution:
no determiner before n1
determiner before n2 is required
the preposition cannot be to
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrases: pattern (2)
(2)v n1 p n2 v p n2 n1
(verb)
If “p n2” is an indirect object of v, then it could
be switched with the direct object n1.
had/v a program/n1 in/p place/n2
had/v in/p place/n2 a program/n1
Determiner before n1 is required to prevent
“n2 n1” from forming a noun compound.
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrases: pattern (3)
(3)v n1 p n2 p n2 * v n1
(verb)
“*” indicates a wildcard position (up to
three intervening words are allowed)
Looks for appositions, where the PP has
moved in front of the verb, e.g.
I gave/v an apple/n1 to/p him/n2
to/p him/n2 I gave/v an apple/n1
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrases: pattern (4)
(4)v n1 p n2 n1 p n2 v
(noun)
Looks for appositions, where “n1 p n2” has
moved in front of v
shaken/v confidence/n1 in/p markets/n2
confidence/n1 in/p markets/n2 shaken/v
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrases: pattern (5)
(5)v n1 p n2 v PRONOUN p n2
(verb)
pronoun
n1 is a pronoun verb (Hindle&Rooth, 93)
Pattern (5) substitutes n1 with a dative pronoun
(him or her), e.g.
put/v a client/n1 at/p odds/n2
put/v him at/p odds/n2
PARC, Aug 3, 2006
Paraphrases: pattern (6)
(6)v n1 p n2 BE n1 p n2
(noun)
to
be
BE is typically used with a noun attachment
Pattern (6) substitutes v with a form of to be (is
or are), e.g.
eat/v spaghetti/n1 with/p sauce/n2
is spaghetti/n1 with/p sauce/n2
PARC, Aug 3, 2006