Li6 Phonology and Morphology

Download Report

Transcript Li6 Phonology and Morphology

Li6 Phonology and
Morphology
Morphological features
Today’s topics
compositional vs holistic morphology
 evidence for morphemes being
composed of (semantic and
grammatical) features
 some uses of morphological features

Holistic or compositional?

Semantics



Holistic (Bock and Levelt 1994)
Componential (Dell 1986)
Morphology

Whole-word representations (Butterworth ‘92, Bybee ‘95)
• Why?
• Reductionist/null hypothesis

Morphological composition (Taft & Forster 1975, Levelt
1989)
• Why?
• general tenet that the brain avoids redundant information storage
• Account for productivity
Semantic features
Problems

Fuzzy categories

we tend to think of concepts expressed by words
and phrases as clear-cut, but…
• what does ‘rich’ mean? ‘tall’? ‘blue’?…
• core/periphery

Operators
some, every, and, or, etc.
 some = SOME, where for any sets X, Y:
SOME (X) (Y) iff X  Y  Ø

 = ‘is a proper subset of’;  = ‘the intersection of’
/kæt/
Gradient Category
Representations
Discrete Category
Representations
Semantic features
Problems: core vs periphery
ostriches
hummingbirds
pigeons
robins
magpies
hawks
storks
penguins
it’s actually easy to model this sort of structure using features
Evidence for semantic
features

Capture generalizations about natural classes (humans, etc.),
subsets


What do the following have in common? girl woman witch governess
dominatrix
Classifiers

Thai uses khon to count [+human] items
• khru lâ j khon ‘teacher three person’ = ‘three teachers’
• ma sí tua ‘dog four body’ = ‘four dogs’


Overt marking needed for morphosyntactic agreement (gender,
number...)
Subcategorization:



admire : ?my cat admires me; *my plants admire me
requires [+human] or [+animate] subject
assassinate: requires [+human, +important, (?)+political] object
Inversion effects

(exact) opposites

good:bad…
 errors (TBD)
Morphological inversion rules


Afro-Asiatic gender polarity (Meinhof 1912)
• Arabic 3-10 take opposite gender of their noun
• θala:θatu bani:na ‘3 (f) sons (m)’
• θala:θu bana:tu ‘3 (m) daughters (f)’
• Somali (non-internal) plurals (Zwicky and Pullum ’83, Lecarme 2002)
• def art: masc /-ka/; fem /-ta/
• agr: Soomáali-ga ‘the Somali’ (m) : Soomaalí-da ‘the Somalis’

Spanish theme vowel polarity (Fitzpatrick, Nevins, and Vaux 2004)
• The theme vowel in the present subjunctive is the “opposite” of that found in
the present indicative
Evidence for
morphological features
speech errors
Speech errors

morphological errors



have to went for had to go
have teachen for have taught
semantic substitution errors

insertion or blending of related or opposite words
• bridge of the neck (nose); a tennis athler
• he has to pay her rent (alimony); I really like to – hate to get up in the
morning

tend to preserve grammatical category
• a laboratory in our own computer

tend to preserve grammatical gender if the target utterance requires the
production of a gender-marked element (Vigliocco, Vinson, Indefrey,
Levelt, & Hellwig 2004)
• seems to support gender as a feature, especially if forms of different
phonological form but identical gender pattern together
Verb errors and features

Ashenfelter and Eberhard 200x

Observation
• words sharing semantic features compete for insertion when
encoded in the same local context (Breedin, Saffran, and
Schwartz 1998)

Hypothesis
• In perseverative and anticipatory speech errors, replacement of
simpler form (e.g. GO) by more complex form (JOG) should be
more common

Results
• Hypothesis supported
Evidence for
morphological features
lexical access effects
Lexical access and features

“When one wants to name the
picture of an object, the
structural description of the
picture is used to activate a
conceptual representation
(typically, a bundle of
semantic features).”
(Bachoud-Lévi and Dupoux
2003:163)
Model from Bachoud-Lévi and
Dupoux 2003:181, based on
Levelt et al 1999
The Internal Lexicon
Conceptual Level
Grammatical Level
Word-form Level
Partial access in TOT
states and aphasia

Certain types of grammatical information are retrievable
independent of lexeme; suggests that these items are
stored as independent features in the lexical entry

Burke et al 1991
• Alternate words retrieved in TOTs are of the same grammatical
class, number, and verb tense as the target word

Caramazza and Miozzo 1997
• gender retrievable without access of lexeme in TOTs
• Similar effect for gender found with some aphasics (Caramazza)
• Cf. grammatical gender of distractor affecting production of German
phrases composes of article + picture name—longer latency with
gender mismatch (Schriefers and Teruel 2000)

Bachoud-Lévi and Dupoux 2003
• Numerals, days, and months spared vs matched controls
• Cf also category-specific deficits (animals, vegetables, tools)
N vs V in aphasia

Rapp and Caramazza 2002

double dissociation of grammatical category vs
modality in single aphasic
• greater difficulty speaking nouns vs verbs
• greater difficulty writing verbs vs nouns

supports the representation of grammatical
category distinctions at post-semantic levels of
representation and processing
Evidence for
morphological features
priming effects
Semantic priming
Meyer & Schvaneveldt 1971
First
Word
bread
bread
bread
bread
castle
castle
Second
Word
butter
cheese
mouse
castle
gate
path
Reaction
Time
855
msec
867
970
1030
871
950
Reaction-time pattern suggested semantic organization of mental lexicon
Historical changes
semantic change
morphologisation
syncretism
Morphologisation in ASL
Frishberg, Nancy. 1975. Arbitrariness
and iconicity: historical change in
American Sign Language. Language
51.3:696-719.
Modeling semantic change

Loss/addition of semantic feature(s)




Parallel to regularization in morphology
Murder, swarthy
murder : ‘kill’ > ‘murder’
Features added to the lexical entry for
murder :




[+human victim]
[+unlawful]
[+intentional]
[+violent]
Analogical leveling


Chaucer, Troilus and Criseyde III.1009-1012
My goode, myn, not I for-why ne how
That Jalousye, allas! that wikked wivere,
Thus causeless is cropen in-to yow;
The harm of which I wolde fayn delivere!
[ne ‘not’, wivere ‘wyvern, viper’]
Chaucer, The Romaunt of the Rose 247-251
Envye.
And by that image, nygh y-nough,
Was peynt Envye, that never lough,
Nor never wel in herte ferde
But-if she outher saugh or herde
Som greet mischaunce, or greet disese.
[peynt ‘painted’, herte ‘heart’, ferde ‘seemed’]
Analogical leveling
plural: eyen
kine
 past:
clomb
crope
lough
yold
holpen








eyes
cows
climbed
crept
laughed
yielded
helped
What is analogy?

Traditional view:

dog : dogs ::
eye : X
Our view of this type of analogy
a. [plural]
 /-n/
/
class 1
/-rən/ /
class 2
/-Ø/ /
class 3
/-z/
/
elsewhere
b. stage I
{eye, noun, class 1}
stage II
{eye, noun}
 Here analogy = loss of exceptional marking.

Syncretism
The Latin declensions
from Halle and
Vaux 1998
Nom
Acc
Gen
Dat
Loc
Instr
Abl
Erg
oblique
-
-
+
+
+
+
+
-
structural
+
+
+
+
-
-
-
+
superior
+
-
-
+
-
+
+
+
free
+
-
+
+
-
-
+
-
1 (f)
2 (m, n)
3 (mfn)
4 (m, n)
5 (f)
Nom
-a
-u-s/m
-s, -Ø
-u-s
-e-s
Acc
-a-m
-u-m
-e-m
-u-m
-e-m
Gen
-a-e
-i
-i-s
-u-s
-e-i
Dat
-a-e
-o
-i
-u-i
-e-i
Abl
-a
-o
-e
-u
-e
Nom
-a-e
-i/-a
m. –es
m. -u-s
-e-s
Acc
-a-s
-o-s
n. -a
n. -u-a
Gen
-a-r-um
-o-r-um
-um
-u-um
-e-rum
Dat/Abl
-i-s
-i-s
-i-bu-s
-i-bu-s
-e-bu-s
The Latin declensions
Nom
Acc
Gen
Dat
Loc
Instr
Abl
Erg
oblique
-
-
+
+
+
+
+
-
structural
+
+
+
+
-
-
-
+
superior
+
-
-
+
-
+
+
+
free
+
-
+
+
-
-
+
-
Some uses of
morphological features
Gender agr with mixed pls

hierarchy of gender agreement with mixed plurals
in Sanskrit (e.g. {Rama and Sita} went to the
store):
m + n  masculine
 m + f  masculine
 n + f  neuter

Underspecification and
competition in DM
(McGinnis 1996)
Impoverishment
strong
[-neuter]
[+neuter]
[-plural]
^
-t
[+plural]
weak
[-plural]

Sauerland 1995 on Norwegian




-e
[+plural]
adjectival endings differ in strong vs weak syntactic positions (see
(10))
in the strong set, -e appears to be the default
by hypothesis, it’s not accidental that -e is default in the strong
context, and also appears everywhere in the weak context
DM analysis:
/t/  [ _ , -pl, +neut] / Adj + _
/^/  [ _ , -pl, -neut] / Adj + _
[“^” = Ø?]
/e/  elsewhere / Adj + _
Impoverishment rule: [±neuter]  Ø / weak context
This rule blocks –t and -^ from being inserted, since each has a gender
specification
• Therefore the default is inserted in all such cases
•
•
•
•
•
Harley and Ritter 2002

McGinnis 2005


“languages without a dual category conflate the dual with the plural.
Likewise…languages without an inclusive category conflate the inclusive with first
person.”
H&R 2002 (from McG 2005)

“the morphosyntactic features of a given language are subject to MINIMAL
CONSTRASTIVE UNDERSPECIFICATION: only contrastive features appear in the
underlying representation, while non-contrastive features are filled in by default rules
(H&R 498; see also Rice and Avery 1995, Brown 1997). H&R propose that a
language lacking a dual category has only [Grp] in the underlying representation,
while [Min] is filled in by a default rule when [Grp] is absent (H&R:489). Thus the
underlying representation of the system in (6a) is as in (8): the singular category has
no number features, while the dual/plural category has only the feature [Grp].”
References
Anne-Catherine Bachoud-Lévi A1 and Emmanuel Dupoux. 2003. An influence of syntactic and semantic variables on word form retrieval. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20.2:163-188.
Badecker, W. and Alfonso Caramazza. 1991. Morphological composition in the lexical output system. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8(5), 335-367.
Bock, K. and W. Levelt. 1994. Language production: grammatical encoding. In M. Gernsbacher, ed., Handbook of psycholinguistics, pp. 945-978. San Diego: Academic Press.
Burke, D., D. MacKay, J. Worthley, and E. Wade. 1991. On the tip of the tongue: what causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language 30:542-579.
Butterworth, B. 1992. Disorders of phonological encoding. Cognition 42:261-286.
Calabrese, Andrea. 1995. Syncretism Phenomena in the Clitic Systems of Italian and Sardinian Dialects and the Notion of Morphological Change. In Jill Beckman, ed., Proceedings of NELS 25, pp.
151-173.
Calabrese, Andrea. 1998. Some Remarks on the Latin Case system and its development in Romance. In J. Lema and E. Trevino, eds., Theoretical Advances on Romance Languages, Amsterdam:
John Benjamins, pp. 71-126.
Calabrese, Andrea. 2002. On Impoverishment and fission in the verbal morphology of the dialect of Livinallongo. In Christina Tortora (ed.) Studies on Italian Dialects. Oxford: Oxford University Press,
pp.3-33.
Clark 1973
Dell, Gary. 1986. A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review 93:283-321.
Fitzpatrick, Justin, Andrew Nevins, and Bert Vaux. 2004. Exchange Rules and Feature-value Variables. Presented at The 3rd North American Phonology Conference, Concordia University, Montréal,
Québec.
Franks, Steven. 1995. Parameters of Slavic Morphosyntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Greenberg, Joseph. 1967. The first (and perhaps only) non-linguistic distinctive feature analysis. Word 23.1:214-220.
Gvozdanović, Jadranka. 1991.Syncretism and Paradigmatic Patterning of Grammatical Meaning. In F. Plank, ed., Paradigms. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, pp. 133–160.
Halle, Morris & Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale & S.J. Keyser, eds., The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain
Bromberger, 111-176. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Halle, Morris and Bert Vaux. 1998. Theoretical Aspects of Indo-European Nominal Morphology: The Nominal Declensions of Latin and Armenian. In Mír Curad: Studies in Honor of Calvert Watkins,
ed. Jay Jasanoff, H. Craig Melchert and Lisi Oliver. Innsbrucker Beitraege zur Sprachwissenschaft, Innsbruck, 223-240.
Halle, Morris. 1997. Distributed morphology: Impoverishment and fission.' In MITWPL 30: Papers at the Interface, ed. Benjamin Bruening, Yoonjung Kang and Martha McGinnis. MITWPL,
Cambridge, 425-449.
Halle, Morris. 2003. The Latin noun declension. Manuscript, MIT.
Harley, Heidi & Elizabeth Ritter. 2002. Person and number in pronouns: A feature-geometric analysis. Language 78, 482-526.
Harris, James W. 1991. The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry 22, 27-62.
Hawkins, Roger & Cecilia Yuet-hung Chan. 1997. The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The ‘failed functional features hypothesis’. Second Language
Research 13:3, 187-226.
Jakobson, Roman. 1962. Beitrag zur allgemeinen Kasuslehre. Gesamtbedeutungen der russischen Kasus. In Selected Writings vol. 2, Mouton, The Hague and Paris, pp. 23–71.
Jakobson, Roman. 1984.The structure of the Russian verb. In L. R. Waugh & M. Halle (Eds.), Russian and Slavic Grammar Studies 1931-1981. Berlin: Mouton.
Janda, Richard and Brian Joseph. 1992. Pseudo-agglutinativity in Modern Greek verb inflection and “elsewhere”. CLS 1:251-66.
Janda, Richard. 1987. On the motivation for ...typology of sound structur[e]...Doctoral dissertation, UCLA.
Katz, Jerold and Jerry Fodor. 1963. The structure of a semantic theory. Language 39:170-210.
Lecarme, Jacqueline. 2002. Gender “polarity”: theoretical aspects of Somali nominal morphology. In Many Morphologies, Paul Boucher and Marc Plénat, eds., 109-141. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla
Press.
Levelt, W. 1989. Speaking: from intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Marcus, Brinkman, Clahsen, Wiese, and Pinker (1995). German inflection: The exception proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 189-256.
McCreight, Katherine and Catherine Chvany. 1991. Geometric Representation of Paradigms in a Modular Theory of Grammar. In F. Plank, ed., Paradigms. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, pp. 91–111.
McGinnis, Martha. 1995. Fission as feature-movement. In MITWPL 27: Papers on Minimalist Syntax, ed. Robert Pensalfini & Hiroyuki Ura. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, 165-187.
Meinhof, Carl. 1912. Die Sprachen der Hamiten. Hamburg: Abhandlungen des Hamburgishcen Kolonialinstituts.
Miozzo, Michele and Alfonso Caramazza. 1997. Retrieval of Lexical-Syntactic Features in Tip-of-the-Tongue States. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
23.6:1410-1423.
Neidle, Carol. 1988. The Role of Case in Russian Syntax. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Noyer, Rolf. 1997. Features, Positions and Affixes in Autonomous Morphological Structure. Garland Publishing, New York. Revised version of 1992 MIT Doctoral Dissertation.
Rapp, Brenda and Alfonso Caramazza. 2002. Selective difficulties with spoken nouns and written verbs: a single case study. Journal of Neurolinguistics 15:373-402.
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Indefrey, P., Levelt, W. J. M., & Hellwig, F. 2004. Role of grammatical gender and semantics in German word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory and Cognition, 30, 483–497.
Wunderlich, Dieter. 2002. Is There Any Need for the Concept of Directional Syncretism? Manuscript, Universität Düsseldorf. To appear in L. Gunkel, G. Müller, and G. Zifonun, eds., Explorations in
Nominal Inflection. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Zwicky and Pullum. 1983. Phonology in syntax: The Somali optional agreement rule. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 1, Number 3.