Transcript Chapter 12
+
Chapter 12
Memory in Childhood
+
2
Infantile Amnesia
Infantile Amnesia:
The tendency for adults to have few autobiographical
memories from below the age of 5
Studying
infantile amnesia can be difficult, as it is:
Hard to verify memories from childhood -- researchers focus
on dateable, verifiable events, such as the birth of a sibling
Hard to know whether childhood memories are genuine
recollections or are reconstructed from stories and
photographs
Genuine memories tend to be more visual, less verbal,
more emotional, more complete (Crawley & Eacott, 2006)
+
3
Infantile Amnesia
Sheingold and Tenney (1982):
Participants:
College students and
children (ages 4–12)
Task:
Answer specific questions
about a sibling’s birth from
when they were 3–11 years
old
e.g. “Who took care of
you while your mother
was in the hospital?”
Mothers were asked the
same questions
Results:
If the birth occurred after 3
years old, very little forgetting
occurred
Even if it occurred many
years ago
If birth occurred before 3
years, virtually nothing was
remembered
+
4
Mnemonic Abilities in Infancy
Infants’ linguistic skills are highly
limited, so:
Experimenters can’t use verbal
instructions
Tasks require motor, rather
than verbal responses
But infants’ motor responses
are also limited
It is hard to establish whether
they are consciously aware of
their memories
i.e. are these declarative or
implicit memories?
Memories are considered declarative,
provided they pass two filters
(Richmond & Nelson, 2007):
Amnesia Filter:
If an amnesiac can do the task,
then it’s implicit.
If not, then it’s declarative.
Parameter Filter:
If the memory is affected by
factors known to influence
declarative tasks in adults, then
it’s also declarative, such as:
Changes in study time
Retention interval
Contextual changes
+
5
Mnemonic Abilities in Infancy
Implicit Memory in Development
Infants display some (limited) mnemonic abilities almost immediately
after birth
DeCasper and Fifer (1980):
Task:
3-day-olds learned that sucking on a pacifier activated a tape
recording with the voice of either:
The infant’s mother
A stranger
Results:
Infants sucked on the pacifier more when it activated the
familiar voice of their mother
Querleu et al. (1984) replicated the results in infants 2 hours old
Conclusion:
Newborns have the ability to remember both their mother’s
voice and the action that produces the sound of her voice
+
6
Mnemonic Abilities in Infancy
Declarative (Explicit) Memory Development
Declarative
memory abilities develop later than
implicit memory.
Originally,
infants were assumed to lack explicit
(declarative) memory.
This view has changed in light of recent data.
It is now assumed that declarative memory is possible in
children far younger than once thought.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=lPJiB-oGMN0
+
7
Declarative Memory Development
Rovee-Collier’s (1989) Mobile Conjugate
Reinforcement Paradigm
Conjugate means “paired.”
Guiding Principle: Avoid
underestimating baby’s memory
by using what interests them
e.g. a colorful mobile hanging
over a baby’s crib
Attach it to the baby’s foot
with a ribbon
When the baby kicks, the
mobile moves
They are rewarded when
they kick by the mobile’s
movement
Quickly learn to kick in the
mobile’s presence via
operant conditioning
Three phases of the paradigm:
Baseline:
Record how often the baby kicks
when the foot isn’t attached to the
mobile
Learning:
Infants learn that kicking (response)
causes the mobile to move
(reinforcement)
Test:
The ribbon is detached from the
mobile (no reinforcement during
test)
If they kick more than baseline in
the presence of the mobile, they
remember the connection
The retention interval between
learning and test can be
manipulated
+
8
Declarative Memory Development
Rovee-Collier’s (1989) Mobile Conjugate
Reinforcement Paradigm
Rovee-Collier et al.’s (1980)
results:
At short delays:
After 2 days:
Both 2 and 3-month-olds
showed evidence of
retention
2-month olds were back at
baseline
After a week:
3-month olds still show a
reliable effect
Presenting a reminder (a moving
mobile) before testing reactivated
kicking:
After a 2-week delay: Retention
bounced back up to its initial levels
After a 1-month delay: Still
significant kicking behavior
+
9
Declarative Memory Development
Rovee-Collier’s (1989) Mobile Conjugate
Reinforcement Paradigm
The learned kicking behavior is quite specific:
Perceptual discrimination:
If the babies were trained on a mobile with yellow blocks, they
wouldn’t respond to a mobile with metal butterflies instead
However, if they’re trained on many different mobiles, they
would then generalize the kicking response to novel mobiles
It is as if they learned the mobile “concept”
Context-sensitivity:
If an infant was trained in a crib but tested in the kitchen, they
wouldn’t kick
If the crib’s décor was changed, the amount of kicking would be
reduced
+
10
Declarative Memory Development
The Mobile Conjugate Reinforcement Paradigm
Is it declarative memory?
Probably (Rovee-Collier, 1997):
Infants’ performance is
determined by factors that are
more important in declarative
than implicit memories, e.g.:
Participant’s age
Retention interval
Context (what the mobile and
the environment looks like)
The Mobile Conjugate
Reinforcement paradigm isn’t
suitable for infants over 7
months, so:
Hartshorn and Rovee-Collier
(1997) introduced a similar
task for older infants:
Infants instead learn to
press a lever to make a
miniature train move
They demonstrate memory
by pressing the lever even
when the train no longer
moves
+
11
Mnemonic Abilities in Infancy
Deferred Imitation
Background:
Meltzoff (1985)
Meltzoff’s novel toy
Piaget believed that infants
Participants:
didn’t possess the ability to do
14-month-olds
deferred imitation until
24-month-olds
2+ years
Exposure Conditions:
Deferred Imitation Task:
Imitation: Observe experimenter
pull the toy apart
Experimenter produces a
sequence with objects
Control: Observe experimenter
move the toy in a circle
e.g. use a mallet to hit a
metal plate
Baseline: Give the novel toy to
the infant without pre-exposure
Delay of varying
lengths
Delay:
The infant then
Wait 24 hours before infant is
tries to imitate the
given the toy
action
+
12
Mnemonic Abilities in Infancy
Deferred Imitation
Results:
14-month-olds
45% of infants in the
experimental condition
imitated the experimenter’s
action
Only 7.5% of infants in the
baseline/control conditions
imitated
24-month-olds
70% of infants in the
experimental condition
imitated
Follow-up Experiments:
Collie and Hayne (1999)
6-month-olds remember
around 20% of the actions
they saw 24 hours earlier
Bauer et al. (2000)
60% of 16-month-olds
produced actions in the
right order after a 12-month
delay
+
13
Deferred Imitation
Evidence of Declarative Memory?
Is
deferred imitation declarative or implicit memory?
Evidence
that deferred imitation requires
declarative/explicit memory:
Adult amnesiacs show little evidence of deferred imitation
(McDonough et al., 1995)
Amnesiacs are selectively impaired in declarative memory
Preverbal infants who imitated were later able to verbalize
their performance
Only declarative memories are likely accessible to
language
+
14
Principles of Memory Development
Hayne (2004)
Older infants typically encode/store information faster than younger infants
Older infants remember information over longer delays
e.g. 18-month-olds kick in the mobile conjugate reinforcement paradigm up to 12
months after training; 6-month-olds only remember for about 2 weeks
Older infants make use of a greater variety of retrieval cues (their memories
are more flexible)
e.g. 6-month-olds require twice as much exposure to do deferred imitation,
compared to 12-month-olds
e.g. 18-month-olds still imitated when the toy was changed slightly; younger infants
did not (Hayne, Boniface, & Barr, 2000)
Forgotten memories can be retrieved when a reminder is presented
3 minutes of exposure to the experimenter moving the mobile 1 day before testing
vastly extended the retention interval during which they exhibited memory
+
15
Developmental Cognitive
Neuroscience
Numerous developments during the first 2 years of life contribute to
better mnemonic abilities:
Attention improves
Language starts to be acquired
World knowledge accumulates
The brain is developing
Schacter and Moscovitch (1984)
Implicit memory is controlled by a memory system likely present at birth
Striatum
Cerebellum
Brainstem
Declarative memories depend on a late-developing memory system
Reaches maturity between 8–10 months
+
16
Changes at the Neural Level
More myelination
More interconnections
More neurons in the
hippocampus and in
prefrontal areas
Newborn
6 Months
2 Years
+
17
Developmental Cognitive
Neuroscience
Brain regions underlying declarative memory continues to develop
well into childhood
Medial Temporal Lobe
Hippocampus
Dentate gyrus—develops for 1 year after birth
Other parts not fully developed until 2–8 years
Parahippocampal complex
Prefrontal cortex
Synaptic density increases until 24 months
Not fully matured until around 20 years
Axons in the central nervous system
Continue to myelinate over the first year
Speeds up processing and learning
+
18
Changes in Brain by Age
+
19
Sensitive Periods in Development
Neural changes correlate with sensitive periods in
development and important milestones.
+
20
Developmental Cognitive
Neuroscience
Benefits of the Approach
Does not simply describe
changes
Helps to explain how and
why changes arise
Offers a partial
understanding of the
differences between types of
memory
e.g. declarative vs. implicit
Limitations to the Approach
Often relies heavily on
correlations between the
rate of brain maturation and
behavioral performance
But correlation does not
imply causation!
+
21
Further Memory Developments
Siegler (1998)
Declarative memory continues to improve into childhood, due to a
number of inter-related factors:
Basic capacity in short-term memory/working memory increases over the
years
Subvocally rehearse faster/more
Adopt better strategies
Learn and use new strategies (e.g. rehearsal)
Accumulate more knowledge
Helps form schemas to organize memories
Develop better metamemory:
Knowledge about one’s own memory and how it works
Helps children select the best strategy to use
+
22
Candid Camera Demo
CD – Remember This Message
+
23
Further Memory Developments
Basic Capacity
Gathercole et al. (2004) studied working memory (WM) development between
the ages of 4–15 years
WM Component
Test
Phonological Loop
Verbal Storage
(Digit Span)
Central Executive
Complex Memory
Span (Backward
Digit Recall)
Visuo-Spatial
Sketchpad
Visuo-Spatial
Memory (Visual
Pattern Recall)
Results:
All three components developed over time
WM structure remained fairly constant over
time
From Gathercole et al. (2004). Copyright © American Psychological Association.
Reprinted with permission.
+ Further Memory Developments
Content Knowledge
Older children possess more
knowledge
Memory performance is
often better when the
learner has a relevant
schema for new knowledge
Adapted from Chi (1978).
Results:
Adults performed better at digit recall
Children performed over 50% better at chess recall
Schneider et al.’s (1993) Follow-Up:
Children and adults of equal chess expertise
performed equally
Both performed better than chess novices
Conclusion:
Memory for chess positions depends largely on
expertise, rather than age
Chi (1978)
Participants:
10-year-old chess
experts
Adult chess novices
Task:
Digit recall and
reproduction of chess
positions
24
+
25
Further Memory Developments
Memory Strategies
Older children are more likely
than younger ones to employ
memory strategies
Evidence comes from
categorized list recall:
Task:
Participants are randomly
presented with
words/pictures from
various categories
They are then asked to
free recall the items
Results:
Adults (Weist, 1972)
Rehearse words by category
Words are recalled by category
(i.e. they “clustered”)
Organizational strategies lead to
better recall
Children aged 8–17
(Schneider, Knopf, & Stefanek,
2002)
Older children used more sorting
strategies
Older children clustered more
Both these strategies increased
steadily over development
+
26
Older Children Use More Sorting
Strategies
(a) Free recall and (b) sorting during learning and clustering during recall assessed by ratio
of repetition (RR) at ages 8, 10, 12, and 17. From Schneider et al. (2002). Copyright ©
American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission.
+
27
Further Memory Developments
Metamemory
Metamemory knowledge increases over development
e.g. younger children tend to drastically overestimate their memory
span (Yussen & Levy, 1975)
Metamemory knowledge is a moderate predictor of memory
performance, R =.41 (Schneider & Pressley, 1989)
A robust metamemory helps children to select appropriate learning
strategies
The correlation isn’t perfect because children might not be
motivated to use their strategies
+
28
Verbatim and Gist Memory
Brainerd and Reyna (2004)
Verbatim Memory
Contain accurate and
detailed information about
to-be-remembered stimuli
Reflects the actual
experience
Improves over childhood
Gist Memory
General semantic information
about to-be-remembered stimuli
Reflects a general
understanding of an
experience
Improves over childhood
Children grow to extract
more meaning from
information
e.g. they start to
spontaneously
categorize items like
adults
+
29
When Improvements Go Wrong
Brainerd and Reyna (2004)
Older children form more gist memory traces
This is generally advantageous, but this occasionally leads
to errors when:
The learning task leads older children to produce more
gist memories than younger children do
The memory test requires verbatim recall/recognition
Greater gist memory increases the likelihood of false
recall/recognition of information very similar in meaning
to the to-be-remembered information
This has largely been studied with the Deese–Roediger–
McDermott (DRM) paradigm – false memory paradigm
+
30
Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM)
Paradigm
Task:
Participants presented with a list of
words
e.g. NURSE, SICK, HOSPITAL,
and PATIENT
All words are related to a
missing target word (e.g.
DOCTOR)
Participants are then asked to
recognize words they saw before,
including the missing word
Result of Interest:
How often do people mistakenly
recall/recognize having seen the
missing target word?
Developmental Results:
False recall/recognition increases
progressively during childhood
From Brainerd and Reyna (2004).Copyright © 2004 Elsevier.
Reproduced with permission.
Conclusion:
Older children are better at semantic
processing
So they tend to categorize all the words
under the missing word, which becomes
highly active
+
31
Declarative vs. Implicit Memory
Development
Implicit memory is pretty well established at
birth and does not seem to improve with age
There are a few discrepant results, however.
Declarative memory starts off less developed,
but it then begins to improve rapidly.
Russo et al. (1995) compared implicit to
declarative memory development.
Task:
Asked children to ID degraded (or intact)
pictures of objects
Implicit measure:
Perceptual priming
Declarative measure:
Free Recall
Implicit
Declarative
Data from Russo et al. (1995).
+
32
Declarative vs. Implicit Memory
Development
Why
the general lack of implicit improvement?
Compared to declarative memory, implicit memory involves
more basic processes.
Implicit memory is relatively unaffected by other cognitive
skills, such as:
WM capacity
Content knowledge
Strategy
Metamemory
+
33
Autobiographical Memory in Infancy
Can infants store autobiographical memories?
Nelson (1988)
Tape-recorded the “crib talk” of Emily between the ages of 21 and 36
months
Results:
At 21 months, Emily was recalling (often fragmented) events
Most from the previous day
Some from up to 6 months before!
At 24 months, Emily was:
Constructing explicit rules and generalizations
Making speculations about the future
At 36 months, Emily stopped talking to herself in the crib
Conclusion:
2-year-olds can encode and remember specific episodes
Caveat:
This was only a single child
+
34
Autobiographical Memory in Infancy
Can infants store autobiographical memories?
Fivush, Gray, and Fromhoff
(1987)
Interviewed ten children
(average age = 33 months)
and their parents about
recent and distant events
The children were able to
answer over 50% of
questions about both types of
events
Best recall for
activities/objects, rather
than people/location
A major factor determining how
much can be remembered:
Whether they possessed
language skills to talk about
the event at the time it
happened
At longer delays (4+ years)
before recall, the infant’s age at
encoding is highly important:
If under 2, generally no
memory
If 3, then 50% recall
If 4+, then nearly all recalled
+
35
Autobiographical Memory in Infancy
Simcock and Hayne’s (2003) “Magic Shrinking
Machine”
Memory in young children is
typically assessed by verbal
report
However, they have limited
verbal skills
Research based on verbal
report likely underestimates
young children’s memory
Simcock and Hayne developed
the “magic shrinking machine”
task to address this issue
They included nonverbal
memory tests
“Magic Shrinking Machine”
Participants:
Children age 24–48
months
Task:
Children saw large objects
go into a machine, but
small objects come out
After 24 hours, children
were given three memory
tests:
Verbal recall
Nonverbal photograph
recognition test
Behavioral reenactment
+
36
The Magic Shrinking Machine
Simcock and Hayne (2003)
From Simcock and Hayne (2003). Copyright © American Psychological Association. Reproduced with permission.
Simcock and Hayne’s nonverbal tests revealed hidden memory
retention by:
Relying less on language
Providing more retrieval cues to the children
+
37
Explanations for Infantile Amnesia
Fivush and Nelson’s (2004) Social Cultural Theory
Pre-linguistic memories are hard to express later using language.
Language skills at the time of an event dictate what they can recall
subsequently (Simcock & Hayne, 2002).
Children whose parents have an elaborative reminiscing style
later report more and fuller childhood memories.
Nelson’s (1989) Museum Study:
When mother–child conversations about the museum trip were freely
interacting, rather than practical, the children remembered more.
+
38
Explanations for Infantile Amnesia
Repression
Freud (1915/1917)
Proposed that infantile amnesia occurs through repression, with
threat-related thoughts being:
Banished to the unconscious mind
Transformed into more innocuous memories called “screen
memories”
Problems with the theory:
No evidence supports it
Fails to explain why adults cannot remember positive or neutral
events from childhood
+
39
Explanations for Infantile Amnesia
Cognitive Self Approach
Howe and Courage (1997)
Must have a sense-of-self to form autobiographical memories.
Develops around 2 years.
Visual-self recognition: recognizing one’s reflection in the
mirror
Provides a schema for autobiographical memories.
Evidence (controlling for language):
Self-recognizers had better memory for personal events.
Pre-self recognizers never had good autobiographical memory.
Why can 2-year-olds remember events for months, but not into
adulthood?
Howe and Courage argue that these memories aren’t rehearsed
much.
+
40
Explanations for Infantile Amnesia
Fivush and Nelson’s (2004) Social Cultural Theory
Nelson’s
(1989) Museum Study:
Elaboration provides the children with ample opportunities to
rehearse their own memories.
An elaborative style is more common in Western Cultures.
First memories in Western cultures tend to be earlier and
are more elaborated and emotional than in many Eastern
cultures.
This could also be because Western children are more
inclined to talk about their personal experiences .
+
41
Explanations for Infantile Amnesia
Summary
The cognitive self approach and the social cultural theory both
have supportive evidence and are not mutually exclusive.
The onset of autobiographical memory could depend on the
emergence of self.
Subsequent memory expression is heavily influenced by social,
cultural, and linguistic factors.
Most research into infantile amnesia relies on correlational
evidence.
Causality is difficult to prove under these circumstances.
+
42
Children as Witnesses
Accuracy
Are traumatic events more memorable than
nontraumatic ones?
Not terribly (Cordón et al., 2004):
Both are influenced by age, delay, and
nature of the event
Are children more suggestible than adults?
Yes (Ceci, Baker, & Bronfrenbrenner,
1988):
Younger children are more biased than
are older children by leading
questions:
Questions that carry with them an
implication as to the correct answer
10- to 12-year-olds are no more
suggestible than adults
The Effect of Leading Questions
+
43
Children as Witnesses
Suggestibility
Thompson, Clarke-Stewart, and Lepore (1997) found that young children’s
responses are largely consistent with the view of their questioner.
The responses of 5 to 6-year-olds to questions about potential abuse when
questioned by:
Neutral interviewers:
Are generally accurate
Accusatory interviewers:
Are biased in favor of guilt
Exonerating interviewers:
Are biased in favor of innocence
Young children continue to reflect the prior influence even when:
Questioned by a new, non-suggestive interviewer.
Warned that the previous interviewer may have been mistaken.
+
44
Children as Witnesses
Suggestibility
Young children are suggestible because of their:
Social compliance
They yield to authority figures
They lack social support to stand up for their views
Cognitive incompetence
They come to believe their distorted reports because of limitations
in:
Processing
Attention
Language abilities
Inability to source monitor
They often confuse real-life and television events.
+
45
Children as Witnesses
How to Maximize Accuracy
Reduce social compliance
Avoid leading questions at any point in the questioning process
Garven, Wood, and Malpass (2000)
Train effective source monitoring techniques
Thierry and Spence (2002)
Reinstate the encoding context
According to the encoding specificity principle, memory should be
maximal when the encoding context and the retrieval context match
Priestley, Roberts, and Pipe (1999)
Use nonverbal recall techniques
Asking children to draw what they remember before asking for a verbal
report can elicit idiosyncratic retrieval cues and nonverbal information
Gross and Hayne (1999)
Children remembered 30% more in the drawing condition, which
only increased (without adding errors) at longer delays