Design of Engineering Experiments Part 5 – The 2k

Download Report

Transcript Design of Engineering Experiments Part 5 – The 2k

Design of Engineering Experiments
Part 5 – The 2k Factorial Design
• Text reference, Chapter 6
• Special case of the general factorial design; k factors,
all at two levels
• The two levels are usually called low and high (they
could be either quantitative or qualitative)
• Very widely used in industrial experimentation
• Form a basic “building block” for other very useful
experimental designs (DNA)
• Special (short-cut) methods for analysis
• We will make use of Design-Expert
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
1
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
2
The Simplest Case: The 22
“-” and “+” denote the low and
high levels of a factor,
respectively
• Low and high are arbitrary
terms
• Geometrically, the four runs
form the corners of a square
• Factors can be quantitative or
qualitative, although their
treatment in the final model
will be different
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
3
Chemical Process Example
A = reactant concentration, B = catalyst amount,
y = recovery
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
4
Analysis Procedure for a
Factorial Design
• Estimate factor effects
• Formulate model
– With replication, use full model
– With an unreplicated design, use normal probability
plots
•
•
•
•
Statistical testing (ANOVA)
Refine the model
Analyze residuals (graphical)
Interpret results
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
5
Estimation of Factor Effects
A  y A  y A
ab  a b  (1)


2n
2n
 21n [ab  a  b  (1)]
B  yB  yB
ab  b a  (1)


2n
2n
 21n [ab  b  a  (1)]
See textbook, pg. 209-210 For
manual calculations
The effect estimates are:
A
= 8.33, B = -5.00, AB = 1.67
Practical interpretation?
Design-Expert analysis
ab  (1) a  b
AB 

2n
2n
 21n [ab  (1)  a  b]
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
6
Estimation of Factor Effects
Form Tentative Model
Model
Model
Model
Model
Error
Error
Term
Effect
SumSqr
% Contribution
Intercept
A
8.33333
208.333
64.4995
B
-5
75
23.2198
AB
1.66667
8.33333
2.57998
Lack Of Fit 0
0
P Error
31.3333
9.70072
Lenth's ME
Lenth's SME
Chapter 6
6.15809
7.95671
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
7
Statistical Testing - ANOVA
The F-test for the “model” source is testing the significance of the
overall model; that is, is either A, B, or AB or some combination of
these effects important?
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
8
Design-Expert output, full model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
9
Design-Expert output, edited
or reduced model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
10
Residuals and Diagnostic Checking
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
11
The Response Surface
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
12
The 23 Factorial Design
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
13
Effects in The 23 Factorial Design
A  y A  y A
B  yB  yB
C  yC   yC 
etc, etc, ...
Analysis
done via
computer
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
14
An Example of a 23 Factorial Design
A = gap, B = Flow, C = Power, y = Etch Rate
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
15
Table of – and + Signs for the 23 Factorial Design (pg. 218)
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
16
Properties of the Table
• Except for column I, every column has an equal number of + and –
signs
• The sum of the product of signs in any two columns is zero
• Multiplying any column by I leaves that column unchanged (identity
element)
• The product of any two columns yields a column in the table:
A  B  AB
AB  BC  AB 2C  AC
• Orthogonal design
• Orthogonality is an important property shared by all factorial designs
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
17
Estimation of Factor Effects
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
18
ANOVA Summary – Full Model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
19
Model Coefficients – Full Model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
20
Refine Model – Remove Nonsignificant Factors
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
21
Model Coefficients – Reduced Model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
22
Model Summary Statistics for Reduced Model
• R2 and adjusted R2
SSModel 5.106 10
R 

 0.9608
5
SST
5.314 10
5
2
2
Adj
R
SS E / df E
20857.75 /12
 1
 1
 0.9509
5
SST / dfT
5.314 10 /15
• R2 for prediction (based on PRESS)
PRESS
37080.44
2
RPred  1 
 1
 0.9302
5
SST
5.314 10
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
23
Model Summary Statistics
• Standard error of model coefficients (full
model)
se( ˆ )  V ( ˆ ) 
2
MS E
2252.56


 11.87
k
k
n2
n2
2(8)
• Confidence interval on model coefficients
ˆ  t / 2,df se( ˆ )    ˆ  t / 2,df se( ˆ )
E
Chapter 6
E
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
24
The Regression Model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
25
Model Interpretation
Cube plots are
often useful visual
displays of
experimental
results
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
26
Cube Plot of Ranges
What do the
large ranges
when gap and
power are at the
high level tell
you?
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
27
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
28
The General 2k Factorial Design
• Section 6-4, pg. 227, Table 6-9, pg. 228
• There will be k main effects, and
k 
  two-factor interactions
 2
k 
  three-factor interactions
 3
1 k  factor interaction
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
29
6.5 Unreplicated 2k Factorial Designs
• These are 2k factorial designs with one
observation at each corner of the “cube”
• An unreplicated 2k factorial design is also
sometimes called a “single replicate” of the 2k
• These designs are very widely used
• Risks…if there is only one observation at each
corner, is there a chance of unusual response
observations spoiling the results?
• Modeling “noise”?
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
30
Spacing of Factor Levels in the
Unreplicated 2k Factorial Designs
If the factors are spaced too closely, it increases the chances
that the noise will overwhelm the signal in the data
More aggressive spacing is usually best
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
31
Unreplicated 2k Factorial Designs
• Lack of replication causes potential problems in
statistical testing
– Replication admits an estimate of “pure error” (a better
phrase is an internal estimate of error)
– With no replication, fitting the full model results in zero
degrees of freedom for error
• Potential solutions to this problem
– Pooling high-order interactions to estimate error
– Normal probability plotting of effects (Daniels, 1959)
– Other methods…see text
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
32
Example of an Unreplicated 2k Design
• A 24 factorial was used to investigate the
effects of four factors on the filtration rate of a
resin
• The factors are A = temperature, B = pressure,
C = mole ratio, D= stirring rate
• Experiment was performed in a pilot plant
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
33
The Resin Plant Experiment
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
34
The Resin Plant Experiment
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
35
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
36
Estimates of the Effects
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
37
The Half-Normal Probability Plot of Effects
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
38
Design Projection: ANOVA Summary for
the Model as a 23 in Factors A, C, and D
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
39
The Regression Model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
40
Model Residuals are Satisfactory
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
41
Model Interpretation – Main Effects
and Interactions
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
42
Model Interpretation – Response
Surface Plots
With concentration at either the low or high level, high temperature and
high stirring rate results in high filtration rates
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
43
Outliers: suppose that cd = 375 (instead of 75)
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
44
Dealing with Outliers
•
•
•
•
•
•
Replace with an estimate
Make the highest-order interaction zero
In this case, estimate cd such that ABCD = 0
Analyze only the data you have
Now the design isn’t orthogonal
Consequences?
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
45
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
46
The Drilling Experiment
Example 6.3
A = drill load, B = flow, C = speed, D = type of mud,
y = advance rate of the drill
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
47
Normal Probability Plot of Effects –
The Drilling Experiment
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
48
Residual Plots
DESIGN-EXPERT Plot
adv._rate
Residuals vs. Predicted
2.58625
Res iduals
1.44875
0.31125
-0.82625
-1.96375
1.69
4.70
7.70
10.71
13.71
Predicted
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
49
Residual Plots
• The residual plots indicate that there are problems
with the equality of variance assumption
• The usual approach to this problem is to employ a
transformation on the response
• Power family transformations are widely used
y y
*

• Transformations are typically performed to
– Stabilize variance
– Induce at least approximate normality
– Simplify the model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
50
Selecting a Transformation
• Empirical selection of lambda
• Prior (theoretical) knowledge or experience can
often suggest the form of a transformation
• Analytical selection of lambda…the Box-Cox
(1964) method (simultaneously estimates the
model parameters and the transformation
parameter lambda)
• Box-Cox method implemented in Design-Expert
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
51
(15.1)
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
52
The Box-Cox Method
DESIGN-EXPERT Plot
adv._rate
Box-Cox Plot for Power Transforms
A log transformation is
recommended
6.85
Lambda
Current = 1
Best = -0.23
Low C.I. = -0.79
High C.I. = 0.32
The procedure provides a
confidence interval on
the transformation
parameter lambda
5.40
Ln(Res idualSS)
Recommend transform:
Log
(Lambda = 0)
3.95
If unity is included in the
confidence interval, no
transformation would be
needed
2.50
1.05
-3
-2
-1
0
1
2
3
Lam bda
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
53
Effect Estimates Following the
Log Transformation
Three main effects are
large
No indication of large
interaction effects
What happened to the
interactions?
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
54
ANOVA Following the Log Transformation
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
55
Following the Log Transformation
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
56
The Log Advance Rate Model
• Is the log model “better”?
• We would generally prefer a simpler model
in a transformed scale to a more
complicated model in the original metric
• What happened to the interactions?
• Sometimes transformations provide insight
into the underlying mechanism
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
57
Other Examples of
Unreplicated 2k Designs
• The sidewall panel experiment (Example 6.4, pg. 245)
– Two factors affect the mean number of defects
– A third factor affects variability
– Residual plots were useful in identifying the dispersion
effect
• The oxidation furnace experiment (Example 6.5, pg.
245)
– Replicates versus repeat (or duplicate) observations?
– Modeling within-run variability
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
58
Other Analysis Methods for
Unreplicated 2k Designs
• Lenth’s method (see text, pg. 235)
– Analytical method for testing effects, uses an estimate
of error formed by pooling small contrasts
– Some adjustment to the critical values in the original
method can be helpful
– Probably most useful as a supplement to the normal
probability plot
• Conditional inference charts (pg. 236)
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
59
Overview of Lenth’s method
For an individual contrast, compare to the margin of error
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
60
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
61
Adjusted multipliers for Lenth’s method
Suggested because the original method makes too many
type I errors, especially for small designs (few contrasts)
Simulation was used to find these adjusted multipliers
Lenth’s method is a nice supplement to the normal
probability plot of effects
JMP has an excellent implementation of Lenth’s method
in the screening platform
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
62
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
63
The 2k design and design optimality
The model parameter estimates in a 2k design (and the effect estimates) are
least squares estimates. For example, for a 22 design the model is
y   0  1 x1   2 x2  12 x1 x2  
(1)   0  1 (1)   2 (1)  12 (1)(1)  1
a   0  1 (1)   2 (1)  12 (1)(1)   2
b   0  1 (1)   2 (1)  12 (1)(1)   3
ab   0  1 (1)   2 (1)  12 (1)(1)   4
The four
observations
from a 22 design
 0 
 1 
 (1) 
1 1 1 1 
 
 
a
1 1 1 1
 ,β   1  ,ε   2 
y = Xβ + ε, y    , X  
 2 
 3 
b
1 1 1 1
 
 
 


 ab 
1 1 1 1 
 12 
 4 
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
64
The least squares estimate of β is
βˆ = (XX)-1 Xy
The “usual” contrasts
1
(1)  a  b  ab 
 a  ab  b  (1) 


b  ab  a  (1) 


(1)

a

b

ab


 (1)  a  b  ab 


4
 ˆ0 
(1)  a  b  ab   a  ab  b  (1) 
 




ˆ
 1  1  a  ab  b  (1)  

4
 ˆ   I 4 b  ab  a  (1)    b  ab  a  (1) 
 2  4 

 
4
ˆ 

(1)  a  b  ab  


 12 
 (1)  a  b  ab 



4

4
0

0

0
Chapter 6
0
4
0
0
0
0
4
0
0
0 
0

4
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
The XX matrix is
diagonal –
consequences of an
orthogonal design
The regression
coefficient estimates
are exactly half of the
‘usual” effect estimates
65
The matrix XX has interesting and useful properties:
V ( ˆ )   2 (diagonal element of (XX)1 )

2
Minimum possible
value for a four-run
design
4
Maximum possible
value for a four-run
design
|(XX) | 256
Notice that these results depend on both the design that you
have chosen and the model
What about predicting the response?
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
66
V [ yˆ ( x1 , x2 )]   2 x(XX)-1 x
x  [1, x1 , x2 , x1 x2 ]
V [ yˆ ( x1 , x2 )] 
2
(1  x12  x22  x12 x22 )
4
The maximum prediction variance occurs when x1  1, x2  1
V [ yˆ ( x1 , x2 )]   2
The prediction variance when x1  x2  0 is
V [ yˆ ( x1 , x2 )] 
2
4
What about average prediction variance over the design space?
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
67
Average prediction variance
1 1
1
I    V [ yˆ ( x1 , x2 )dx1dx2
A 1 1
A = area of design space = 22  4
1 1
1
2 1
    (1  x12  x22  x12 x22 )dx1dx2
4 1 1 4
4 2

9
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
68
Design-Expert® Software
FDS Graph
Min StdErr Mean: 0.500
Max StdErr Mean: 1.000
Cuboidal
radius = 1
Points = 10000
1.000
StdErr Mean
0.750
0.500
0.250
0.000
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
Fraction of Design Space
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
69
For the
2
2 and
in general the
k
2
• The design produces regression model coefficients that
have the smallest variances (D-optimal design)
• The design results in minimizing the maximum
variance of the predicted response over the design space
(G-optimal design)
• The design results in minimizing the average variance
of the predicted response over the design space (Ioptimal design)
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
70
Optimal Designs
• These results give us some assurance that
these designs are “good” designs in some
general ways
• Factorial designs typically share some (most)
of these properties
• There are excellent computer routines for
finding optimal designs (JMP is outstanding)
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
71
Addition of Center Points
to a 2k Designs
• Based on the idea of replicating some of the
runs in a factorial design
• Runs at the center provide an estimate of
error and allow the experimenter to
distinguish between two possible models:
k
k
k
First-order model (interaction) y   0    i xi    ij xi x j  
i 1
k
k
i 1 j i
k
k
Second-order model y   0    i xi    ij xi x j    ii xi2  
i 1
Chapter 6
i 1 j i
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
i 1
72
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
73
yF  yC  no "curvature"
The hypotheses are:
k
H 0 :   ii  0
i 1
k
H1 :   ii  0
i 1
SSPure Quad
nF nC ( yF  yC )2

nF  nC
This sum of squares has a
single degree of freedom
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
74
Example 6.6, Pg. 248
Refer to the original experiment
shown in Table 6.10. Suppose that
four center points are added to this
experiment, and at the points x1=x2
=x3=x4=0 the four observed
filtration rates were 73, 75, 66, and
69. The average of these four center
points is 70.75, and the average of
the 16 factorial runs is 70.06.
Since are very similar, we suspect
that there is no strong curvature
present.
Chapter 6
nC  4
Usually between 3
and 6 center points
will work well
Design-Expert
provides the analysis,
including the F-test
for pure quadratic
curvature
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
75
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
76
ANOVA for Example 6.6 (A Portion of Table 6.22)
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
77
If curvature is significant, augment the design with axial runs to
create a central composite design. The CCD is a very effective design
for fitting a second-order response surface model
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
78
Practical Use of Center Points (pg. 260)
• Use current operating conditions as the center
point
• Check for “abnormal” conditions during the
time the experiment was conducted
• Check for time trends
• Use center points as the first few runs when there
is little or no information available about the
magnitude of error
• Center points and qualitative factors?
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
79
Center Points and Qualitative Factors
Chapter 6
Design & Analysis of Experiments
7E 2009 Montgomery
80