Causation and the Rules of Inference
Download
Report
Transcript Causation and the Rules of Inference
Causation and the Rules
of Inference
Classes 4 and 5
Arlington Heights and Causal
Reasoning in Law
Claim: Both the Housing Authority (MHDC) and a specific
individual claimed injury based on the Village’s zoning actions
to disallow construction of Lincoln Green, a multi-family
housing development.
Plaintiff asserted an “actionable causal relationship”
between the Village’s action and his alleged injury
Court of Appeals reversed the District Court ruling and held
that the “ultimate effect” of the rezoning was racially
discriminatory, and would disproportionately affect Blacks
Challenge: Was the Village’s zoning ordinance racially
motivated? Was there intent to discriminate?
SCOTUS: Disparate impact is not sufficient evidence to claim
discrimination. Affirmative proof of discriminatory intent is
needed to show Equal Protection violation
Washington v Davis – intent is shown by factors such as:
Facts –
27 African American residents in town of 64,000 in preceding census
Developer had track record of building low-income housing, the Order wanted to
create such housing
Most residents in new housing were likely to be African Americans
Opponents cited likely drop in property values that would follow the construction
Historical context – town had remained nearly all white as areas around it
became economically diverse, thereby limiting access of non-whites to the new
better paying jobs
Court uses a complex causation argument to work around discriminatory
intent
Disproportionate impact
Historical background of the challenged decision
Specific antecedent events
Departures from normal procedures
Contemporary statements of the decision makers
“Rarely can it be said that a[n] “administrative body … made a decision
motivated by a single concern…or even a ‘dominant’ or ‘primary’ one (citing
Washington v Davis)
Re-zoning denial wasn’t a departure from ‘normal procedural sequence’ (565566)-- ??
How would you prove the claim that there was a discriminatory intent that
produced a disparate impact? How would you prove it with certainty?
Causal Reasoning
Elements of causation in traditional positivist
frameworks (Hume, Mill, et al.)
Correlation
Temporal Precedence
Constant Conjunction (Hume)
• Cause present-cause absent demand
• Threshold effects – e.g., dose-response curves (Cranor at
18)
Absence of spurious effects
Challenges
Indirect causation
Distal versus proximal causes temporally
Leveraged causation
Multiple causation versus spurious causation
Temporal delay
Modern causal reasoning implies a dynamic relationship, with
observable mechanisms, not just a set of antecedent
relationships and correlations. Why does the light go out when
we throw the switch? Why does the abused child grow up to
become an abuser? How do fetuses exposed to Bendectin
develop birth defects? Why did people stop committing
suicide in the UK in the 1950s when the gas pipes were
sealed off?
Valid causal stories have utilitarian value
Causal theories are essentially good causal stories
Causal mechanisms are reliable when they can support
predictions and control, as well as explanations
We distinguish causal description from causal
explanation
We don’t need to know the precise causal mechanisms to
make a “causal claim
Instead, we can observe the relationship between a
variable and an observable outcome to conform to the
conceptual demands of “causation”
Criteria for Causal Inference
Strength (is the risk so large that we can easily rule out other
factors)
Consistency (have the results have been replicated by different
researchers and under different conditions)
Specificity (is the exposure associated with a very specific
disease as opposed to a wide range of diseases)
Temporality (did the exposure precede the disease)
Biological gradient (are increasing exposures associated with
increasing risks of disease)
Plausibility (is there a credible scientific mechanism that can
explain the association)
Coherence (is the association consistent with the natural history
of the disease)
Experimental evidence (does a physical intervention show
results consistent with the association)
Analogy (is there a similar result to which we can draw a
relationship)
Source: Sir Austin Bradford Hill, The Environment and Disease: Association or Causation, 58 Proc. R.
Soc. Med. 295 (1965)
Alternate Paths: Experimental v.
Epidemiological Causation
Experiments test specific hypotheses through
manipulation and control of experimental
conditions
Epidemiological studies presumes a probabilistic
view of causation based on naturally occurring
observations
• Challenges of observational studies? (Cranor at 31)
“A’s blow was followed by B’s death” versus “A’s
blow caused B’s death”
We usually are striving toward a “but for” claim,
and these are two different pathways to ruling in or
out competing causal factors
Errors in Causal Inference
Two Types of Error
Type I Error (α) – a false positive, or the probability of
falsely rejecting the null hypothesis of no relationship
Type II Error (β) – a false negative, or the probability
of falsely accepting the null hypothesis of no
relationship
The two types of error are related in study design,
and one makes a tradeoff in the error bias in a study
Statistical Power = 1 – β -- probability of correctly
rejecting the null hypothesis
In regulation, we care more about false
negatives
Medication
What about in criminal trial outcomes? Both Type I
and Type II errors are problems.
http://www.intuitor.com/statistics/T1T2Errors.html
Interpreting Causal Claims
In
Landrigan, the Court observes that
many studies conflate the magnitude of
the effect with statistical significance:
Can still observe a weak effect that is
statistically significant (didn’t happen by
chance)
Can observe varying causal effects at
different levels of exposure, causal effect is
not indexed
Facts
Asbestos case
• 16 years of exposure to asbestos to 1972
• Diagnosed in 1981, died a year later
Medical expert didn’t examine decedent, but based
his opinion on epidemiological studies
Expert was epidemiologist, not physician, but that
didn’t rule out his testimony
• “Key to admission of opinion is expert’s reasoning and
methodology”
• Court has to distinguish scientifically sound reasoning from
self-validating expert….personal beliefs”
Wagoner testified that diet is implicated in colon
cancer, but not smoking, arthritis, or (moderate)
alcohol use.
Multiple causation
Court distinguished between clinical studies –
experiments – and statistical analysis of
diseases in groups of patients. “The statistical
associations may become so compelling…that
they raise a legitimate implication of causation”
(Landrigan at 1085)
They really are talking about causation criteria
Court also cites importance of consistency of
such associations with other knowledge – e.g.,
consistent with known biological mechanisms
(implies integration of clinical and
epidemiological evidence
Statistical significance – courts demand p < .05
(i.e., results were obtained by chance), equating
scientific standard with legal standard
??? Why .05? General agreement on this?
Alternatives to Statistical Significance
Odds Ratio – the odds of having been exposed given the
presence of a disease (ratio) compared to the odds of not having
been exposed given the presence of the disease (ratio)
Risk Ratio – the risk of a disease in the population given
exposure (ratio) compared to the risk of a disease given no
exposure (ratio, or the base rate)
Attributable Risk – proportion of the disease that is statitically
attributed to the factor, a “composite measure of …the relative
risk…if exposed…and the proportion of the population so exposed”
(Black and Lilienfeld at 761).
(Rate of disease among the unexposed – Rate of disease among the exposed)
(Rate of disease among the exposed)
Effect Size versus Significance
Such indicia help mediate between statistical significance and
effect size, which are two different ways to think about causal
inference
Can there be causation without significance? Yes
• Allen v U.S. (588 F. Supp. 247 (1984)
• In re TMI, 922 F. Supp. 997 (1996)
Thresholds
Asbestos Litigation – relative risk must exceed 1.5,
while others claim 2.0 relative risk and 1.5 attributable
risk
• RR=1.24 was “significant” but “…far removed from proving
‘specific’ causation” (Allison v McGhan, 184 F 3d 1300 (1999))
• See Marder v Searle at 1092 (1986)
Probability standard seems to be at 50% causation, or
a risk ratio of 2.0 (“ a two-fold increase” – Marder v GD
Searle, 630 F. Supp. 1087 (1986)).
Landrigan at 1087 – 2.0 is a “piece of evidence”, not a
“password” to a finding of causation
If RR < 2.0, still could support causation if
accompanied by individual clinical data on exposure
and disease
• But exclusion of evidence at a RR=1.0 risks a Type II error,
depending on study design
How Strong is the Study?
Several study design considerations that bear on
‘strength’ – selection bias, measurement error, etc. (next
class)
Sensitivity – proportion of individuals screened for a
causal characteristic who are correctly classified as
having that characteristic (false positives)
Specificity – proportion of individuals who do no have the
characteristic and are correctly classified as such (false
negatives)
Not unlike Type I and Type II errors, in that there often is
a tradeoff
The higher the RR, the greater the odds of a Type II error
Foundational Requirements for
Causal Inference*
Theory – should lead to observables
Replicability – transparency of theory, data and method
Control for Rival Hypotheses and “Third Factors”
Pay Attention to Measurement
Validity and Reliability
Relevance of Samples, Size of Samples, Randomness
of Samples, Avoid Selection Bias in Samples
Statistical Inferences and Estimation – use triangulation
through multiple methods
Research should produce a social good
Peer review contributes to evolution of theory
Research data should be in the public domain via data archiving
* See, Epstein and King
Case Study
Pierre v Homes Trading Company
Lead paint exposure in childhood produced behavioral and social
complications over the life course, resulting in criminal activity and
depressed earnings as an adult
Specific injuries – plumbism, elevated lead levels in blood and bones,
mental abnormalities, etc., including “propensity for delinquency” that
resulted in lost future earnings of $5M
Plaintiff’s scientific evidence – epidemiological study of birth cohort exposed
to lead paint in childhood and their future criminality and life outcomes
See: Deborah Denno, Biology and Violence (1990): sample of 487
boys in Philadelphia studied from age 0 to age 22, over 3,000 variables
that were analyzed to identify correlations between lead exposure,
criminality and incarceration
Elevated blood-lead levels were the strongest predictor of disciplinary
problems in school and the third-strongest predictor of juvenile crime.
One of the other two strongest predictors of juvenile crime, previous
disciplinary problems, also can be traced to lead exposure during
infancy and childhood
See: Rhode Island v Lead Industries Association et al., 2001 R.I. Super.
Lexis 37
See: Lutter and Mader, Litigating Lead-Based Paint Hazards, Chapter 4
in Regulation through Litigation (Kip Viscusi, ed.) 106 (2002)
Illustrating Complex Causation