שקופית 1 - Tel Aviv University

Download Report

Transcript שקופית 1 - Tel Aviv University

Correlations Without
Synchrony
Carlos D. Brody
Presented by:
Oded Ashkenazi
1
Overview
•
•
•
•
•
•
Neurological Background
Introduction
Notations
Latency, Excitability Covariograms
3 Rules of Thumb
Conclusion
2
Neurological Background
The Human Brain:
A Complex
Organism
3
Neurological Background
• 1011 neurons (x50 the number of people on
earth)
5
10
• Each one is connected with
synaptic
connections
16
• Total of 10 Synaptic Connections
4
Neurological Background
5
6
Neurological Background
input
CPU
output
7
Neurological Background
Spike Trains – plots of the spikes of each neuron
as a function of time
Raster Plot – a plot of a few Spike Trains
simultaneously
8
Neurological Background
• Histograms - a plot of the binned data as a
function of time.
9
Neurological Background
• PSTH - peri-stimulus time histogram
is a Histogram of stimulated neurons lined
up by the stimulus marker. (marks the
beginning of the stimulus).
• The PSTHs give some measure of the
firing rate or firing probability of a neuron
as a function of time.
10
11
Neurological Background
•
Crosscorrelogram - is a function which
indicates the firing rate of one neuron
versus another.
•
It's pretty simple to compute the
crosscorrelogram. The problem is how
to interpret it.
12
Neurological Background
13
Neurological Background
• The crosscorrelogram provides some indication of
the dependencies between the two neurons.
14
Introduction
• Peaks in spike train correlograms are usually
taken as indicative of spike timing
synchronization between neurons.
• However, a peak merely indicates that the two
spike trains were not independent .
• Latency or excitability interactions between
neurons can create similar peaks.
15
Introduction
• On each trial, most spikes in cell 1 have a
corresponding, closely timed spike in cell 2.
16
Introduction
• The two spike trains were generated independently.
But the overall latency of the response varies
together over trials.
17
Introduction
• The spikes for the two cells were generated
independently. but the total magnitude of the
response varies together over trials
18
Notations
The spike trains of two cells will be represented by
two time-dependent functions, S1(t) and S2(t).
The cross-correlogram of each trial (r) is:
19
Notations
• Let
represent averaging over trials r.
The PSTH of Si is:
20
Notations
• When you stimulate the cells that you're
recording from, you increase their firing rates.
• If you do this simultaneously in both cells you've
introduced a relationship between the firing
probabilities of the cells.
• The Covariogram removes the peak in the
original correlogram that was due to costimulation of the cells.
21
Notations
The covariogram of S1 and S2 is:
R
K
R - raw cross-correlogram
K - shuffle corrector (shift predictor)
22
23
Notations
• The expected value of V is zero:
• Significant departures of V from zero indicate
that the two cells were not independent
24
Notations
• Estimating the significance of departures
of V from 0 requires some assumptions:
– S1 is independent of S2.
– Different trials of S1 are independent of
each other.
– Different bins within each trial of S1 are
independent of each other.
25
Notations
The variance of V is:
Where:
and
are the mean and variance
of
over r trials.
and
are the number of trials in the
experiment.
26
Spike Timing Covariations.
• How Spike Timing covariations lead to a
peaked covariogram.
27
Latency Covariations
• Lets consider the responses of two
Independent neurons.
• For each trial r, take the responses of
both neurons and shift both of their spike
trains, together by some amount of time tr
28
Latency Covariations
How will it affect the covariogram ?
• The raw correlogram R will not be
affected.
R
K
• The shuffle corrector K will be affected
because the PSTHs are broadened by
the temporal jitter introduced by the shifts
tr.
29
Latency Covariations
• The latency shifts will make K broader,
and therefore shallower, while having no
effect on R.
• The width and shape of the peak in V are
largely determined by the width and
shape of the peak in R.
30
Latency Covariations
• How latency covariations lead to a
peaked covariogram.
31
Excitability Covariations
Consider a cell whose response can be
characterized as the sum of a stimulusinduced response plus a background
firing rate.
• Z(t) is the typical stimulus-induced firing
rate.
• “gain” factors, and , represent possible
changes in the state of the cell.
32
Excitability Covariations
• Suppose the 2 cells only interaction is
through their gain parameters.
• What is their covariogram ?
• Reminder:
33
Excitability Covariations
• The shape of V will be the shape of the
corrector K:
• K has a width determined by the width of
peaks in the cell’s PSTHs
• The amplitude of V will be given by:
34
Excitability Covariations
• An easily computable measure of
excitability covariations is the integral
(sum) of the covariogram:
• It is proportional to the covariation in the
mean firing rates of the two cells
35
Excitability Covariations
• How Excitability covariations lead to a
peaked covariogram.
36
Rules of Thumb
• There are three major points in
comparison to latency and excitability
covariations:
– Autocovariograms
– Covariogram shapes
– Covariogram integrals
37
Autocovariograms
• Autocovariograms: This function lets
you discern the fine time structure, if any,
in the spike train of a single neuron
• Spike Timing autocovariograms are flat
and not at all similar to the crosscovariogram. Unlike the ones of
Excitability or Latency.
38
Autocovariograms
Latency
Spike Timing
Excitability
39
Covariogram shapes
• Spike timing covariogram shapes are
much more arbitrary than Latency or
Excitability covariogram shapes.
• Latency and Excitability shapes are tied
to the shapes of the PSTHs
• Spike timing shapes are not.
40
covariogram integrals
• Large, positive covariogram integrals
imply the presence of an excitability
covariations component.
• In the spike timing case, the integral, if
positive, will often be small.
41
conclusion
• We want to analyze neuron
synchronization by sampling only a small
number of trials.
• This is a special case of a more general
problem:
Taking the mean of a distribution as
representative of all the points of the
distribution.
42
conclusion
• For this to work: std << mean
• This is common to gene networks, text
searches, network motifs.
• Investigators must interpret means
with care !
43
The End
Questions ?
Thank you for listening.
44