The prosody of finiteness and non-finiteness

Download Report

Transcript The prosody of finiteness and non-finiteness

The prosody of finiteness and
non-finiteness:
the accent of Estonian finite and
non-finite verbs
Anne Tamm
[email protected]
RIL HAS Budapest
University of Florence
Prosody and finiteness
• This contribution is about the prosodic
characteristics of finiteness and nonfiniteness.
• Can we regard prosody as another
language module where finiteness
operates in some languages?
Modules and finiteness
• I assume that certain verb forms,
constructions and clauses can be either
finite or non-finite.
• In terms of language modules, previous
research has established that finiteness
and non-finiteness phenomena are found
in morphology, syntax, semantics and
pragmatics. How about prosody?
Morphological, semantic and
pragmatic finiteness overlapping
(1)
Ta
on
siin.
he[nom] be.3s here
‘He is here.’
Morphologically,
non-morphologically non-finite
(2)
Ta ei olnud siin sugugi
he[nom] neg be-nud_ptcp here at all
mõtlemata kõigile tagajärgedele.
without thinking of all the possible
consequences
‘He was not here without thinking
of all the possible consequences.’
Morphologically nonfinite,
non-morphologically finite
(3)
Ta
olevat
kodus.
he[nom] be-quot home-ine
‘Allegedly, he is at home.’
What is finite in
Estonian negative clauses?
• in Example (4) below containing negation,
the verb is morphologically non-finite, but
non-morphologically, it may be regarded
finite
• what is finite and non-finite in this
sentence?
– is ei a particle,
– a verb,
– and if it is a verb, is it finite?
Negation and finiteness
(4)
Ta
ei olnud
he[nom] neg be-nud_ptcp
‘He was not here.’
siin.
here
(I set this apart in the study of finiteness
and accent)
Negation, finiteness, accent, stress
• This talk concentrates on the contrast between
morphological and non-morphological finiteness and
non-finiteness phenomena, adding negation.
• Negation is a crucial an object of study, since
– it allows to test for several interdependent factors operating on
the different levels of linguistic description just as (non)finiteness
– its accentuation and syntactic properties can be teased apart
from those properties of finiteness and non-finiteness that apply
because of the properties of the affirmative
– that there are many types that are present in Estonian allow us
to say something more general about negation, prosody and
(non)-finiteness
The main questions
• Can we tease apart the morphological and nonmorphological finiteness and non-finiteness
phenomena in terms of prosody, and accent?
• Does negation form any patterns with finiteness?
• Is morphological/non-finite negation special in
terms of stress and accent?
• What is the module on which we can build the
prosodic structure?
• Can we predict stress and accent?
Negation, finiteness, syntax
• negated verbs assume a position in a
sentence that can be different from the
affirmative
• the arguments may also change their
positions and accentuation due to the
changed pragmatic and semantic factors
• in what follows, I establish the stress and
accent pattern in some simple sentences
and negation expressions
-mata attracts stress
•
•
in production and perception tests, the -mata
morpheme is distinguished from all other nonfinite verb forms in stress – it is frequently
stressed, while all others are unstressed (tud,
nud, tav, v, mas, mast, ma, da)
it is also bisyllabic, but since there are other
bisyllabic combinations of nonfinite
morphemes and case that are not stressed
(-nuna, -tuna, -tava, -vale), the stress on -mata
can be considered remarkable
-mata attracts accent
Also, while other non-finite forms tend to be
unaccented in an identical position, the –mata
forms are accented (lexical exceptions exist).
(5) Ta
on
(’)ujumas.
he[nom]
be.3sg swim-m_inf_ine
‘He is off swimming.’
(6) Ta
on
’uju’mata.
he[nom]
be.3sg
swim-m_inf_abe
‘He has not swum.’
ei is unaccented
The accentuation of the negation
verb/particle ei is perceived as
acceptable, but strange in perception
tests, triggering unusual contexts.
(7) Ta
ei olnud
siin.
he[nom]
neg be-nud_ptcp here
‘He was not here.’
(accent on olnud or siin)
mitte is partly unpredictable
From among the negation items, the
accent of mitte is the most unpredictable
and depends most on its position and the
information structure, and its lexical
surroundings.
mitte is partly predictable
‘teps ‘mitte ‘not at all’ (always accented)
mitte ‘mina vaid ‘sina (never accented)
(‘)mitte ‘midagi ‘nothing at all’ (variably
accented)
eba- depends on the lexicalization
The stress pattern in the combinations with the
prefixal eba- seem to depend on the
lexicalization of the combination (the adjective
part has always stress if not lexicalized, and
nouns tend to be stressed).
‘ebameeldiv (-meel- has no stress)
‘eba‘uuenduslik (-uuen- has stress)
‘eba’küdoonia ‘quince’ (-küdoonia has stress)
Finite verbs unaccented
The finite verb in a clause is mostly
unaccented, unless it is not required by
information structural or other
constraints, and its accenting is
perceived as strange.
(8) Ta
on
’siin.
he[nom] be.3sg here
‘He is here.’
Clause-final finite forms accented
Clause-final finite forms are accented.
(9)
Seda
ta mulle eile
‘ütles.
This.part s/he I-all yesterday say.3s.pst
‘Yesterday s/he did say it to me.’
Negative verbs are accented
•
•
However, some listable finite verbs are
rather accented, and their accentuation
can be attributed to their role in
discourse.
Semantically negative verbs tend to
attract accent.
eemaldama ‘remove’ vs lisama ‘add’
loobuma ‘decline’ vs nõustuma ‘agree’
eitama ‘negate’
Non-finite forms clause-final
Non-finite forms appear mostly clausefinally, and are accented.
(10)
Seda
sai mulle eile
‘öeldud.
This.part s/he I-all yesterday say.tud-ptcp
‘Yesterday it was told to me.’
Non-finite forms as main predicates
in finite clauses
Non-finite forms that are the main
predicates in finite clauses are not
accented (exactly as the finite verbs).
(11) Ta
olevat ’kodus.
he[nom] be-quot home-ine
‘Allegedly, he is at home.’
Negated verb, variable accent
Whether the verb form combining with ei
is accented or not, depends on its
position and its discourse properties
(accented if clause-final, and/or on the
‘list’ of preferably accented predicates)
(12) Ei tea ‘öelda (no accent on tea ‘know’)
Neg know say-da_inf
‘One cannot say.’
Verbs in negative sentences
vary in accent
The accentuation of the negation
verb/particle ei is perceived as
acceptable, but strange in perception
tests, triggering unusual contexts.
(13) Ta
ei (’)olnud
’siin.
he[nom]
neg be-nud_ptcp here
‘He was not here.’
(accent on olnud or siin)
Non-finite negated forms as main
predicates in finite clauses
Non-finite forms that are the main
predicates in finite clauses and that are
negated are accented.
(11) Ta
ei ’olevat kodus.
he[nom] neg be-quot home-ine
‘Allegedly, he is at home.’
Accenting is a significant difference
between morphologically finite and nonfinite negated main predicates.
Clause-final verbs
are accented
Clause-final verbs are accented.
(14)
Seda
ta
mulle eile
ei ‘öelnud.
This.part s/he I-all yesterday neg
say-nud_ptcp
‘Yesterday s/he did not say this to me.’
Non-morphological finiteness as
the basis for accenting
• One can conclude that the type of
finiteness that is relevant for accenting is
non-morphological in Estonian.
Prosody patterns little with
morphological finiteness
• Prosodic patterns are generally more
regular in terms of their correlation with the
(non-)finitness phenomena in syntax,
semantics, pragmatics, and extra-linguistic
or semi-linguistic properties (accentuation
at the end of an utterance/sentence) than
in terms of morphological finiteness.
An interesting correlation
• the correlation of accent and morphological
finiteness is weak with morphological finiteness
but some cases stand out as exceptions
• the negative (abessive) morphologically nonfinite predicates (mõtlemata) and negated
morphologically non-finite main predicates (ei
olevat) are regularly accented
• the negative (abessive) morphologically nonfinite forms can be predicted to carry accent, and
the stress on the morpheme
Final conclusions
• the examples above showed that it is
rather the negative content that attracts
stress and accent in Estonian
• non-finiteness and negation together
predict accent in some well defined
environments in Estonian
Prosody not a module where
finiteness operates
• Can we regard prosody as another
language module where finiteness
operates?
• The answer is no for Estonian, but there
are regularities that might help to tease
apart the morphological, syntactic,
semantic and pragmatic finiteness.
Bibliography
to copy and paste to see better
Asu, E.-L. 2004. The phonetics and phonology of Estonian intonation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Cambridge, UK. Dissertation. Abari
K, Tamm A., Gábor K., Olaszy G. 2007. Számítógépes összehasonlító szövegelemzés ügyfélszolgálati tájékoztatók legfontosabb prozódiai elemeinek a
meghatározására. Magyar Számítógépes Nyelvészeti Konferencia. Szerk.: Alexin Zoltán és Csendes Dóra. Szegedi Tudományegyetem Informatikai
Tanszékcsoport. Szeged. 24-33. Cinque, G. 1993. "A null theory of phrase and compound stress", /Linguistic Inquiry/ 24. pp.239-97. Ehala, M. 2001.
Eesti keele baassõnajärjest. Keele kannul : pühendusteos Mati Erelti 60. sünnipäevaks 12. märtsil 2001. Publications of the Department of Estonian of the
University of Tartu ; 17 24-41. Erelt, M., Metslang, H. (2006). Estonian clause patterns - from Finno-Ugric to Standard Average European. Linguistica
Uralica, XLII(4), 254 - 266. Grosz, B., Hirschberg, J. 1992. “Some intonational characteristics of discourse structure”, Proceedings of ICSLP-92, Banff,
pp. 429-432. Help, T. 1996 "Free" word order : Finnish vs. Estonian and Hungarian. Linguistica Uralica 1 321-30. Hayes, B., 1995. Metrical stress theory:
principles and case studies Chicago, Ill. : University of Chicago Press. Huumo, T. 1993."Suomen ja viron kontrastiivista sanajärjestysvertailua". In YliVakkuri, Valma (ed.): Tutkielmia syntaksin ja pragmasyntaksin alalta. Studia comparativa linguarum orbis Maris Baltici 1. Publications of the Department
of Finnish and General Linguistics, University of Turku 43. Turku, p. 97-158. Huumo, T. 1994. "Näkökulmia suomen ja viron sanajärjestyseroihin".
Sulkala, Helena & Laanekask, Heli (eds.): Lähivertailuja 8, Suomalais-virolainen kontrastiivinen seminaari Hailuodossa 7.-9.5.1994. : Research reports
from the Department of Finnish and Saami, Univeristy of Oulu 40. p. 21 39. Huumo, T. 1995. "On the Position of Subject in Finnish and in Estonian" XXI
Kielitieteen päivät Oulussa 6. 7.5.1994. Acta Universitatis Ouluensis Humaniora B 19. Oulu 1995. p. 79 87.Huumo, T. 1996. "Remarks on the Functions of
Word Order in Finnish and in Estonian". Congressus Octavus Internationalis Fenno-Ugristarum. Jyväskylä 10.-15.8.1995. Pars IV: Sessiones sectionum
Syntaxis et Semantica & Contactus linguistici et status hodiernus linguarum & Cetera linguistica. Eds. Heikki Leskinen, Sándor Maticsák & Tõnu
Seilenthal. Jyväskylä. S. 47 51. É. Kiss, K., 1995. Discourse Configurational Languages. Oxford University Press, 1995. É. Kiss, K., Kiefer, F., Siptár,
P.: Új magyar nyelvtan. Budapest: Osiris, 1998. Komlósy, A. (1989): Fókuszban az igék, In: Általános Nyelvészeti Tanulmányok, 17. Kornai A., Kalman,
L. 1988. Hungarian Sentence Intonation. Harry van der Hulst, Norval Smith Linguistic Models 11. Autosegmental studies on pitch accent. Foris.183-195.
Ladd, D., R., Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996. Lehiste, I. 2007. The Finno-Ugric Prosody Project. Linguistica
Uralica XLIII 1. Lindström, L. 2006 Infostruktuuri osast eesti keele sõnajärje muutumisel Keel ja Kirjandus 2006, 11. Lindström, L. 2005. Finiitverbi
asend lauses. Sõnajärg ja seda mõjutavad tegurid suulises eesti keeles. Tartu : Tartu Ülikooli Kirjastus. Mihkla, M. Meister, Eek. 2002. Tekst-kõne
süntees. Keel ja Kirjandus. Mihkla, M.; Kerge, K.; Pajupuu, H., Statistical modelling of intonation and breaks for Estonian text-to-speech synthesizer.
Robert Vich (Ed.). Electronic Speech Signal Processing. Proceedings of the 16th Conference Joined with the 15th Czech-German Workshop "Speech
Processing". Prague, September 26-28. 2005 (91 - 98). Studientexte zur Sprachkommunikation. Band 36. Dresden: TUDpress, 2005. Müürisep, K., Eesti
keele arvutigrammatika: süntaks. Dissertationes Mathematicae Universitatis Tartuensis 22. Tartu 2000. Pajupuu, H.; Tael, K. 1989. The acoustic
parameters and the perception of certain elements of pragmatic structure. The 28th Acoustic Conference on Physiological Acoustics, Psychoacoustics,
Acoustics of Music and Speech. October 3.-6.1989 Strbske Pleso-High Tatras Czechoslovakia,1989. Strbske Pleso-High Tatras Czechoslovakia:, 1989,
298 - 301. Rizzi, L., The fine structure of the left periphery. In: Liliane Haegeman (ed.) Elements of Grammar. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp. 281-337, 1997.
Sahkai, H.1999. Eesti verbifraasi sõnajärg. Keel ja Kirjandus1 24-32. Tael, K. 1988. Infostruktuur ja lauseliigendus.3. 133-143. Tael, K. 1988.
Sõnajärjemallid eesti keeles (võrrelduna soome keelega) : läänemeresoome keeleteaduse sümpoosion (Turku, 30.08-2.09.1988) / Eesti NSV TA
Ühiskonnateaduste osakond, Keele ja Kirjanduse Instituut. Preprint KKI ; 56. Tamm, A. K. Abari, G. Olaszy. 2007. Accent Assignment Algorithm in
Hungarian Based on Syntactic Analysis, in Proceedings of the 8th Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association
(Interspeech 2007), Bonn, pp. 466-469. Tamm, A. K. Abari, G. Olaszy. 2005. Kísérlet automatizált szövegelemzési módszerek kialakítására a
szóhangsúlyok meghatározásához [An attempt to create text analysis methods for determining word stress] in D. Csendes (ed.), III. Magyar
Számítógépes Nyelvészeti Konferencia NSzNy 2005. 2005. december 8-9. Tamm, A. 2008. Problems of automatic accent assignment: a comparison of
Hungarian and Estonian verbs. Traat, M. 2006. Information Structure in Discourse Representation. Computational analysis of theme, rheme and focus
with Unification-based Combinatory Categorial Grammar. PhD thesis, University of Edinburgh. Varga, L.: Intonation and Stress: Evidence from
Hungarian. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, 2002. Varga, L., Rhythmic stress alternation in Hungarian. 1994: 233–254. Varga, L., Hungarian intonation
contours. 2 (1996):111–144. Varga, L., The Intonational Phrase and Secondary Intonational Phrase Formation in Hungarian. 3 (1998):251–263
Acknowledgements
• This research is supported by the grant The
bidirectional relationships between the speech
rhythm and Estonian grammatical and lexical
structures, grant number ETF 2009, ETF7998 of
the Estonian Science Foundation (Eestikeelse
kõne rütmilisuse peegeldused grammatilistes ja
leksikaalsetes struktuurides (ja vice versa)) or
Modelling intermodular phenomena in Estonian,
grant number SF 2009, SF0050023s09 of the
Estonian Science Foundation (Eesti keele
alusuuringud keeletehnoloogiliste rakenduste
teenistuses)