Transcript May 26

Science news
Rest of the film
Science and Society: how society can
inform good science
1.
2.
3.
1.
2.
Gould on 19th century craniometry and
anthropology: class and race differences.
Gould on 19th century craniometry and
psychology: gender/sex differences.
Science and its social context: How society
can affect science
I.
How social context can inform (good)
science
i.
Gould on 19th century craniometry and
anthropology: class and race
differences.
ii. Gould on 19th century craniometry and
psychology: gender/sex differences.
How society can affect science
How social context can inform (good) science
Things we have studied to consider:
I.
II.
III.
IV.
The presence and role of auxiliary
assumptions
The role of systems or bodies of theories or
hypotheses in generating “If H, then I”
The role of paradigms in setting up a
puzzle-solving tradition
The theory-ladenness of observation
How society can affect science
The presence and role of auxiliary
assumptions
II.
The role of systems or bodies of theories or
hypotheses in generating “If H, then I”
III. The role of paradigms in setting up a
puzzle-solving tradition
IV. The theory-ladenness of observation
Are the only relevant auxiliary assumptions,
bodies of theories, paradigms, etc. internal
to science – or do they include social
beliefs?
I.
How social context can inform (good?)
science
S.J. Gould, “Wide Hats and Narrow Minds”

The hypothesis: intelligence is caused by brain
size (larger is better!)

The players: Paul Broca

Founder of The Anthropological Society and
Renowned Craniologist

Players continued: Louis Gratiolet

Comparative anatomist
How social context can inform (good?) science
S.J. Gould, “Wide Hats and Narrow Minds”

The players cont’d: (the late!) Cuvier

The “crucial test”: the size of the great
Cuvier’s brain

The evidence: his hat!

And G. Hevre:

Was there water on Cuvier’s
brain?
How social context can inform (good?) science
Gould’s bottom line:
“On the surface, this tale seems ludicrous. The
thought of France's finest anthropologists
arguing passionately about the meaning of a
dead colleague's hat could easily provoke the
most misleading and dangerous inference of
all about history—a view of the past as a
domain of naive half-wits, the path of history
as a tale of progress, and the present as
sophisticated and enlightened.
How social context can inform (good?) science
Gould’s bottom line:
“But if we laugh with derision, we will never
understand. Human intellectual capacity has
not altered for 50,000 years so far as we can
tell. If intelligent people invested intense
energy in issues that now seem foolish to us,
then the failure lies in our understanding of
their world, not in their distorted
perceptions.”
How social context can inform (good?) science
If intelligent people invested intense energy in
issues that now seem foolish to us, then the
failure lies in our understanding of their
world, not in their distorted perceptions.
What do we “fail to understand” about their world
if we laugh at this example?

Metaphysical commitments to distinct and
significant biological groups: men vs. women,
white Europeans vs. people of color,
intelligent vs. less intelligent.

Social position reflects innate capacities and
traits, not social bias or engineering.
How social context can inform (good?) science
If intelligent people invested intense energy in issues that now seem
foolish to us, then the failure lies in our understanding of their
world, not in their distorted perceptions.
A then current paradigm (or auxiliary assumptions, or
body of theory):
“Among the questions heretofore discussed within the
Anthropological Society, none is equal in interest
and importance to the question before us now. . . .
“The great importance of craniology has struck
anthropologists with such force that many among us
have neglected the other parts of our science in
order to devote ourselves almost exclusively to the
study of skulls. ...
“In such data, we hope to find some information relevant
to the intellectual value of the various human races.”
How social context can inform (good?) science




S.J. Gould, “Women’s Brains”
The hypothesis: Women had smaller
brains than men and, like it or not, could
not equal men in intelligence.
Players: Broca, Le Bon, and others.
The tests: head/skull measuring of
contemporary women in autopsies, and
skull measuring of fossil remains.
How social context can inform (good?) science
From L. Manouvier, Broca’s colleague who rejected
Broca’s conclusions about women:
“Women displayed their talents and their diplomas.
They also invoked philosophical authorities. But
they were opposed by numbers … unknown to
John Stuart Mill.
“These numbers fell on women like sledge hammers,
and they were accompanied by commentaries
and sarcasms more ferocious than the most
misogynist imprecations of certain church
fathers.”
How social context can inform (good?) science
Gould’s argument: some of Broca’s numbers are
impeccable …
“I have the greatest respect for Broca’s meticulous
procedure [in the measurement of autopsied
brains]. His numbers are sound.
But
“But science is an inferential exercise, not a
catalogue of facts. Numbers, by themselves,
specify nothing. All depends on what you do
with them.”
How social context can inform (good?) science
Gould’s conclusions: some of Broca’s numbers are
impeccable but…

What did the women die from whom he
autopsied?

And what age were they as opposed to the
men?

And are the sizes of women’s brains relative
to overall body size the same (or even a bit
larger) than those of men?
How social context can inform (good?) science
Broca’s response to such queries:
We might ask if the small size of the female’s brain
depends exclusively upon the small size of her body
[as some colleagues ask].
But we must not forget than women are, on average, a
little less intelligent than men, a difference which we
should not exaggerate but which is nonetheless real.
Circular reasoning: he was supposed to establish that
women are a little less intelligent then men!
We are therefore permitted to suppose that the relatively
small size of the female brain depends in part on her
physical inferiority and in part on her intellectual
inferiority.
How social context can inform (good?) science
Broca’s study of and conclusion from hominid
remains.

A very small difference between the apparent
sizes of male and female brains (based on skull
size)

Conclusion: the current larger differences in
the sizes of male and female brains are the
result of evolutionary pressures upon
“dominant” males or men, and “passive”
females or women.

Gould: Only 7 male skulls and 6 females skulls, all
from one location and time:
“Never has so little data yielded such far reaching
conclusions.”
How social context can inform (good?) science
Gould, again, trying to understand “their” world,
and his “bottom line” in the essay:
“To appreciate the social role of Broca and his
school, we must recognize that his statements
about the brains of women do not reflect an
isolated prejudice toward a singular
disadvantaged group.
“They must be weighed in the context of a general
theory that supported contemporary social
distinctions as biologically ordained …
How social context can inform (good?) science
Gould and “their” world:
“Women, blacks, and poor people suffered the
same disparagement, but women bore the
brunt of Broca’s argument because he had
easier access to data on women’s brains.
“Women were singularly denigrated but they also
stood as surrogates for other disenfranchised
groups. As [one of Broca’s disciples] wrote,
“Men of the black races have a brain scarcely
heavier that that of white women.”
“I do not regard as empty rhetoric the claim that
women’s battles are for all of us.”
How social context can inform (good?) science
Contemporary lessons?
If we don’t take past scientists to be dimwits, and we
recognize that the human brain hasn’t changed, then
we need to consider how if at all, in our own time and
“world,” social beliefs and context can impact (good)
science.
Although differences in intelligence between the poor and
the well off are no longer directly studied, those
alleged to distinguish the sexes and races continue
(although the latter to less of an extent than in the
1960’s through the 1980’s).
Exhibit A: The Bell Curve
Exhibit B: Larry Summers when president of Harvard!