Transcript ch8
Chapter 8
Experimental
Studies in
Epidemiology
Objectives
Discuss the role of randomization in controlled trials
Discuss the role of blinding in controlled trials
Identify the general strengths and weaknesses of
controlled trials
Identify the advantages to using a run-in design, a
factorial design, a randomized matched pair
design, or a group-randomized design
Discuss some of the ethical issues associated with
experimental studies
Epidemiologic Experimental Studies
Resemble controlled experiments performed in
scientific research
Best reserved for relatively mature questions
Best for establishing cause-effect relationships and
for evaluating the efficacy of prevention and
therapeutic interventions
Experimental Studies
Also called intervention studies
Investigators influence the exposure of the study
subjects
Two types of experimental trials
Controlled trials
Community trials
Between-Group Design
The strongest methodological design is a betweengroup design, where outcomes are compared
between two or more groups of people receiving
different levels of the intervention
Within-Group Design
May be used where the outcome in a single group
is compared before and after the assignment of an
intervention
Strength – Individual characteristics that might confound
an association (e.g., gender, race, genetic susceptibility)
are controlled
Weakness – Susceptible to confounding from timerelated factors such as the media but may be adjusted for
in the analysis
Controlled Trial
The unit of analysis is the individual
A randomized controlled trial in a clinical setting
is referred to as a clinical trial
Community Trial
The unit of analysis is the group or community
An experimental epidemiologic study where one
group of people, or one community, receives an
intervention and another does not is a community
trial
Natural Experiment
In some rare situations in nature, unplanned events
produce a natural experiment
Levels of exposure to a presumed cause differ
among a population in a way that is relatively
unaffected by extraneous factors so that the
situation resembles a planned experiment
Example of Natural Experiment
Screening and treatment for prostate cancer in
the Seattle-Puget Sound area differed
considerably from that in Connecticut during
1987-90
Specifically, prostate-specific antigen testing was
5.39 (95% confidence interval: 4.76 to 6.11)
times higher in Seattle than in Connecticut, and
the prostate biopsy rate was 2.20 (1.81 to 2.68)
times higher than in Connecticut
Example of Natural Experiment
Ten-year cumulative incidences of radical
prostatectomy and external beam radiation up to
1996 were
2.7% and 3.9% for those in Seattle
0.5% and 3.1% for those in Connecticut
Example of Natural Experiment
Did mortality from prostate cancer from 1987 to
1997 differ between Seattle and Connecticut?
The adjusted rate ratio of prostate cancer mortality
during the study period for Seattle vs. Connecticut
was 1.03 (0.95 to 1.11)
In other words, the 11-year follow-up data showed
no difference in prostate cancer mortality between
the two areas, despite much more intensive
screening and treatment in Seattle
Experimental Studies
Experimental studies may be
Between-group designs
Within-group designs
Between Group Design
When a comparison is made between outcomes
observed in two or more groups of subjects that
receive different levels of the intervention, we call
this a between-group design
Within Group Design
When we compare the outcomes observed in a
single group before and after the intervention, it
is called a within-group design
Random Assignment
Random assignment makes intervention and
control groups look as similar as possible
Chance is the only factor that determines group
assignment
Neither the patient or the physician know in
advance which prevention program or therapy will
be assigned
Confounding and sample size
Non-Randomized Study
Also called convenience sample
Concurrent comparison group is allocated by a
non-random process
Assignment
Problems
Not effective at controlling unmeasured confounding
variables
Measured confounding variables; however, may be
adjusted through analytic methods
Advantages and disadvantages of
randomized controlled clinical trials
Advantages of randomization
Eliminates conscious bias due to physician or patient
selection
Averages out unconscious bias due to unknown factors
Groups are “alike on average”
Disadvantages of randomization
Ethical issues
Interferes with the doctor-patient relationship
Blinding
Three levels of blinding
Single blind – Subjects
Double blind – Investigators
Triple blind – Analyses
Single-Blinded Study
In a single-blinded placebo-controlled study, the
subjects are blinded but investigators are aware
of who is receiving the active treatment
Double-Blinded Study
In a double-blind study, neither the subjects
nor the investigators know who is receiving the
active treatment
Triple-Blinded Study
In a triple-blind study, not only are the
treatment and research approaches kept a
secret from the subjects and investigators, but
the analyses are completed in a manner that is
removed from the investigators
Blinding Patients
Why blind patients?
Patients try to get well/please physicians
Minimize potential bias from a placebo effect
A placebo effect is defined as the effect on
patient outcomes (improved or worsened) that
may occur due to the expectation by a patient (or
provider) that a particular intervention will have
an effect
Chronic Severe Itching Study
Forty-six patients randomly assigned to one of
four groups
Treatment
Cyproheptadine HCI
Trimeprazine tartrate
Placebo
Nothing
Itching Score
27.6
34.6
30.4
49.6
Blinding Patients
To blind patients, use a placebo; for example,
Pill of same size, color, shape as treatment
Sham operation (anesthesia and incision) for angina
relief (unethical)
Problems
In non-drug studies may be impossible/unethical
In drug studies if treatment has characteristic side effects
Blinding Patients
More subjective outcomes call for more serious
consideration of placebo
For example, time to death vs. pain relief
Placebos improve comparability of treatment
groups in terms of compliance and follow-up
For example, if patient perceives improvement
because of medication, more likely to remain in study
Blinding physician or outcome
assessing investigator
Best way to avoid unconscious bias is to blind
Physicians – don’t know which patient is taking the
placebo and which patient is taking the drug
Assessors – of the outcome; are not the treating doctors,
and are not told which treatment was used
What if physician blinding is not possible (e.g.,
surgery or radiation trial)?
Problems with Blinding
For non-drug studies, such as those involving
behavior changes or surgery, it may be
impossible or unethical to blind
It may also be problematic to blind in drug
studies where a treatment has characteristic side
effects
Purpose of Experimental Studies
To identify clinical and public health approaches
to solving public health problems (how to prevent
or treat)
Strengths of blinded randomized
controlled clinical trials
Demonstrate cause-effect relationships
May produce a faster and cheaper answer than
observational studies
Only appropriate approach for some research
questions
Allow investigators to control the exposure levels
as needed
Weaknesses of blinded randomized
controlled clinical trials
Often more costly in time and money
Many research questions are not suitable for
experimental designs because of ethical barriers
and because of rare outcomes
Many research questions are not suitable for
blinding
Standardized interventions may be different from
common practice
May have limited generalizability due to the use of
volunteers, eligibility criteria, and loss to follow-up
Early History of Clinical Trials
1600 – East India Company
1747 – James Lind
1835 – P.C.A. Louis, Charite Hospital, Paris
Day bled after onset
1-3
4-6
7-9
Note:
1827
1837
Died
12
12
3
Lived
12
22
16
33,000,000 leeches imported to Paris
7,000 leeches imported to Paris
% Surviving
50
65
84
.
Designing a Clinical Trial
Assembling study cohort
Measuring baseline variables
Choosing comparison group
Assuring compliance
Selecting treatment
Selecting patient population
Selecting outcome (endpoint)
Ethical considerations
Assembling Study Cohort
Inclusion criteria
Broad vs. specific – related to the extent of generalization
Is the outcome rare (e.g., CHD incidence)? Then
recruit from populations at high risk such as males.
Assembling Study Cohort
Exclusion criteria
Define exclusion criteria that will help control error.
Example: An advanced cancer that may be fatal before the end
of the follow-up period in a subject entering a CHD-prevention
study
Exclude those with difficulty in complying
Examples: Alcoholics, psychotic patients, individuals
planning to move out of state
Sample size
Measuring Baseline Variables
Characterize the study cohort
Identifying information (name, address, ID#)
Demographics (age, race, gender, etc.)
Clinical factors
The first table of a final report of any randomized
blinded trial typically compares the level of baseline
characteristics in the two study groups
Measuring Baseline Variables
Consider measuring the outcome variable
Change (appropriate for within-group design)
To assure disease is or is not present at baseline
(appropriate for between-group design)
Measure various predictors of outcomes (e.g.,
smoking habits) to allow for statistical adjustment
Be parsimonious (i.e., keep simple)
Choosing Comparison Group
Not contaminated by treatment
Ideal – possible to blind, usually meaning placebo
used
Status quo vs. new treatment
Assuring Compliance
Calling the day before clinical visit
Providing reimbursement
Adhering to the intervention protocol
Drug or behavioral intervention should be well tolerated
Taking once a day vs. complex schedule
Measuring compliance (self-report, pill counts, urinary
metabolite levels)
Selecting Treatment
What is the research objective?
Are the therapies safe and active against the
disease?
Is there evidence that one therapy is better than
another?
Is the intervention likely to be implemented in a
clinical practice?
Is the intervention “strong” enough to have a
chance of producing a detectable effect?
Selecting Patient Population
Often a compromise between
1. The population most efficient for answering the clinical
question
2. The population best for generalizing the study findings
For example, many CHD-prevention trials do not
include subjects over age 60 because such
elderly subjects might already have extensive
atheriosclerosis of their coronary arteries that
would no longer be responsive to preventive
efforts.
Selecting Outcomes (Endpoints)
Primary endpoint
For example, the primary endpoint for most phase III
clinical trials in HIV disease is an AIDS-defining event
or death
Major AIDS-defining events are: parasitic infections;
fungal infections; bacterial infections; viral infections;
neoplastic disease; HIV dementia; HIV wasting
syndrome
Selection of the “best” endpoint is often
complicated
Surrogate endpoint
Phase I Trial
Unblinded, uncontrolled study with typically fewer
than 30 patients
The purpose of phase I trials is to determine the
safety of a test in humans
Patients in phase I trials often have advanced
disease and have already tried other options
They often undergo intense monitoring
Phase II Trial
Relatively small (up to 50 people) randomized
blinded trials that test
Tolerability
Safe dosage
Side effects
How the body copes with the drug
Also evaluate which types of disease a treatment is
effective against, further assess side effects and
how they can be managed, and reveal the most
effective dosage level
Phase III Trial
Typically much larger and may involve thousands
of patients
These trials typically involve random assignment
and are used to evaluate the efficacy of a new
treatment
Different dosages or methods of administration
of the treatment are often part of the evaluation
Phase IV Trial
A large study conducted after the therapy has
been approved by the FDA to assess the rate of
serious side effects, and explore further
therapeutic uses
Selected special types of
randomized designs
Basic randomized designs
Run-in designs
Factorial design
Randomization of matched pairs
Group randomization
Run-In Design
All subjects in the cohort are placed on placebo (or
treatment), followed for some period of time
(usually a week or two), and then those who have
remained in the study are randomly assigned to
either the treatment or placebo arms of the study
A limitation of this study design is that the subjects
in the cohort at the time of randomization may no
longer reflect the population of interest
Factorial Design
Subjects are randomly assigned to one of four
groups. The groups represent the different
combinations of the two interventions.
Randomization of Matched Pairs
Improves covariate balance on potential
confounding variables
Matched randomization provides more accurate
estimates than unmatched randomization, and
may involve matching on several potential
confounders
Randomization of Matched Pairs
Subjects are matched in pairs according to some
confounding factor (e.g., age, sex, race/ethnicity)
One subject is then randomly assigned the study
group (e.g., a dietary program, a drug) and the
other is assigned to the comparison control
group
Group Randomization
Groups or naturally forming clusters are randomly
assigned the intervention
Groups may involve
Practices
Schools
Hospitals
Communities
Individuals or patients within a cluster are likely to
be more similar to each other compared to those in
other clusters
Summary of Ethical Principles
Benefits – Maximize good
Risks – Avoid doing harm
Subject – Respect for all persons
Society – Fairness to all
Is consent necessary?
Is it necessary to disclose to the subject the fact that they
will be determined by chance?
Should subjects be compensated for injury?
Who should be permitted (or encouraged) to participate in
clinical research?
When and how should a clinical trial be stopped?
Tuskegee Syphilis Study
The Tuskegee study had nothing to do with
medical experiments
No treatment was offered for syphilis
No new drugs were tested, nor were any efforts
made to establish the efficacy of older chemical
treatments
Summary of Ethical Principles
Competent investigators and good research
design lead to a greater likelihood of benefits,
protect subjects from harm, ensure that peoples’
time is not wasted, and their desire to participate
in a meaningful activity not frustrated.
Source: Levine and Labacqz