Cross-linguistic Syntactic Priming Evidence for shared

Download Report

Transcript Cross-linguistic Syntactic Priming Evidence for shared

Neocerebellar Emulation in Language Processing
Giorgos P. Argyropoulos
Language Evolution and Computation Research Unit, University of Edinburgh
[email protected]
1. Introduction
3. Language comprehension in the
cerebellum
Despite the ample evidence gathered during the last decades in support of
the involvement of the cerebellum in language processing, there has been
little attention paid to the identity of the computations underlying such
involvement, especially given that the cerebellum seems to perform a 3.1. What is a Kalman filter?
unitary computation for a wide range of behavioral repertoires.
Emphasizing language comprehension here, I attempt the grounding of a
The Kalman filter (Kalman, 1960) is a dynamical state estimator testing incoming
recently proposed psycholinguistic processor (Pickering & Garrod, 2007) in
data against expectations, compensating for delays and filtering out noise in the
the cortico-cerebellar circuit.
sensory feedback, if the signal can be modeled as the output of a known dynamical
system. A Kalman regulator uses the sensory error to correct the estimate resulting
from the forward model. The Kalman gain determines the extent to which the
sensory residual influences the a priori estimate (i.e., whether emulation tramps the
actual sensory feedback or not).
2. The cerebellum
3.2. The reception of the Kalman filter in cognitive
psychology and psycholinguistics
2.1. The unitary cerebellar computation
4. The cerebellar pseudosyntax
1)
Townsend & Bever (2001): Sentence comprehension
employs both algorithmic (the actual “syntax”) and faster,
preemptive heuristic computations (the “pseudosyntax”); the
latter pertains to “semantic associations” and “syntactic habits”.

Ito (2000): A CNMC might connect to the cerebral loop as
a reliable copy of the thought/language model in the
temporoparietal areas, with the thought/language process being
alternatively conducted by the frontal areas acting on the CNMC
rather than on the temporoparietal areas, adaptively avoiding the
conscious effort needed for the exploration of cortical loci.
2)
McRae et al. (2005): Predictive top-down processing in
sentence comprehension relies on associative lexical relations,
not captured by spreading activation in lexical networks, but by
expectancy-generation mechanisms.

Expectancy-generation is ideally captured by the internal
model computations (e.g., Wolpert et al., 1998)

Cerebellar patients seem to be particularly impaired in the
noun-verb generation task (Fiez et al., 1992; Gebhart et al., 2002), a
lexical association that McRae et al. (2005) particularly
emphasize for expectation-based sentence comprehension.
• Cytoarchitectural (computational) homogeneity, functional heterogeneity
(e.g., Kawato & Gomi, 1992; Wolpert et al., 1998).
• “a unitary principle of architecture, but a localizationist principle of the
connectivity” (Schmahmann, 2000; 206).
3)
Kempson & Cann (2007): “… a natural subsequent step
of routinization… would be to call up the actions associated with
the verb together with those associated with the clitic with a
single lexical look-up mechanism” (Kempson & Cann, 2007)
Doya (1999): “… a cerebellar mapping can work as a
short-cut circuit or a look-up table for a mapping that was
originally developed by the time-consuming cortico-cortical
processing” (Doya, 1999, 970).


The posterolateral cerebellum is involved in constrained
search space in language tasks:
3.3. The neocerebellar KF linguistic processor
• Increased cerebellar activation in FEW condition in stem
completion task (Desmond et al., 1998)
• Spared performance in subordinate category generation
VS poor performance in superordinate category and
antonym generation for posterolateral cerebellars (Gebhart et al.,
The computations of a Cerebellar Cortico-Nuclear Micro-Complex (Ito, 1989). Figure
taken from Ito (2002, p. 281). Long-term depression (LTD) occurs at parallel fiber-toPurkinje cell synapses after conjunctive activation of these synapses together with
climbing fiber-to-Purkinje cell synapses (Ito et al., 1982).
2002; Fiez et al., 1992)
PC: Purkinje Cell. GR: Granule Cell. CC: Cerebellar Cortex. IO: Climbing Fiber.
MF: Mossy Fiber. CN: Cerebellar Nucleus CF: Climbing Fiber. LTD: Long Term
Depression. Hollow circle: Excitatory synapse. Filled circle: Inhibitory synapse.
5. Conclusion
The posterolateral cerebellum might be involved in language
perception processes by implementing Kalman Filter
computations in a way that makes language perception noiseresistant, and automatic, triggering covert, imitative
involvement of production mechanisms in language perception.
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to my supervisors, Prof. Jim Hurford, Dr. Patrick Sturt and Dr. Thomas Bak, as well as to the Pavlos and Elissavet
Papagiannopoulou Foundation for the financial support in my PhD studies.
References
Figure adapted from Argyropoulos (2008). This is a case of (cerebellar) emulation-induced (mirror
neuron-based) covert imitation in language perception.
Argyropoulos, G. (2008). The subcortical foundations of grammaticalization. In Andrew D. M. Smith, Kenny Smith and Ramon Ferrer i Cancho (Eds.), The Evolution of
Language: Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on the Evolution of Language (pp. 10-17). Singapore: World Scientific Press.
Barton, R.A. (2002). How did brains evolve? Nature , 415, 134-135.
Barton, R.A., & Harvey, P.H. (2000). Mosaic evolution of brain structure in mammals. Nature, 405, 1055-1058.
Bloedel, J.R. (1992). Functional heterogeneity with structural homogeneity: how does the cerebellum operate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 666-678.
Decety, J., Sjoholm, H., Ryding, E., Stenberg, G., and Ingvar, D.H. (1990), The cerebellum participates in mental activity: tomographic measurements of regional cerebral
blood flow. Brain Research, 535, 313-317.
3.4. Evidence for the model
Desmond, J.E., Gabrieli, J.D.E., Glover, G.H. (1998), Dissociation of frontal and cerebellar activity in a cognitive task: evidence for a distinction between selection and
research. Neuroimage, 7, 368-376.
Dow, R.S. (1942). The evolution and anatomy of the cerebellum. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 17, 179-220.
Doya, K. (1999). What are the computations of the cerebellum, the basal ganglia and the cerebral cortex? Neural Networks, 12, 961-974.
• Cerebellar activation in error-detection in processing ambiguous sentences that
require reanalysis (Stowe et al., 2004)
A typical cerebellar forward internal model.
Fabbro, F., Moretti, R., & Bava, A. (2000). Language impairments in patients with cerebellar lesions. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 13, 173– 188.
Fiez, J.A., Petersen, S.E., Cheney, M.K., Raichle, M.E. (1992), Impaired non-motor learning and error detection associated with cerebellar damage. Brain, 115, 155-178.
Gebhart, A.L., Petersen, S.E., Thach, W.T. (2002). Role of the posterolateral cerebellum in language. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 978, 318-333.
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 377–435.
•Deficient error-detection in linguistic tasks for cerebellar patient (Fiez et al., 1992)
Imamizu, H., T. Kuroda, S. Miyauchi, T. Yoshioka, M. Kawato. (2003), Modular organization of internal models of tools in the human cerebellum. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100 (9), 5361-5466.
Ito, M. (2002). Historical Review of the Significance of the Cerebellum and the Role of Purkinje Cells in Motor Learning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 978,
273- 288.
2.2. Involvement in language and
cognitive processing
• Neocerebellar expansion parallels prefontalization (Dow, 1942)
• A striking increase in the lateral
cerebellum in hominoids as opposed to
monkeys (MacLeod et al., 2003; Whiting &
Barton, 2003; Barton & Harvey, 2000;
Barton, 2002)
• Reciprocal projections between the
ventrolateral dentate nucleus and
Broca’s area (e.g., Leiner, Leiner, &
Dow,1986; Middleton & Strick, 1994;1998;
Schmahmann & Pandya, 1997)
• Lack of performance optimization in linguistic tasks for cerebellar patient
Ito, M. (2000). Neural control of cognition and language. In A. Marantz, Y. Miyashita, and W. O'Neil (Eds.), Image, language, brain (pp. 149-162). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
(Fiez et al.,1992).
Ito, M. (1984), The Cerebellum and Neural Control. New York: Raven Press.
Ito, M., Sakurai, M., & Tongroach, P. (1982). Climbing fibre induced depression of both mossy fibre responsiveness and glutamate sensitivity of cerebellar Purkinje cells.
Journal of Physiology (Lond.), 324, 113-134.
• Fundamental role of the cerebellum in mental imagery (Decety et al., 1990; Ryding et
al., 1993) = cases of setting the Kalman gain of the Grush emulator (2004) to zero, i.e.,
cases of maximized reliance on the cerebellar internal model)
•Impaired mental imagery and anticipatory planning in a patient following
unilateral cerebellar hemispheric infarction (Leiner et al., 1987).
Kalman, R. E. (1960). "A New Approach to Linear Filtering and Prediction Problems," Transactions of the ASME - Journal of Basic Engineering, 82, 35-45.
Kawato, M. Gomi, H. (1992). A computational model of four regions of the cerebellum based on feedback-error learning. Biological Cybernetics, 68, 95-103.
Kempson, R. & Cann, R. (2007). Dynamic Syntax and dialogue modelling: preliminaries for a dialogue-driven account of syntactic change. Current Issues in Linguistic
Theory, 284, 73-101.
Leiner, H.C., Leiner, A.L., & Dow, R.S. (1986). Does the cerebellum contribute to mental skills? Behavioral Neuroscience, 100(4), 443-54.
Leiner, H.C., Leiner, A.L., & Dow, R.S. (1989). Reappraising the cerebellum: what does the hindbrain contribute to the forebrain? Behavioral Neuroscience, 103(5), 9981008.
MacLeod, C.E., Zilles, K., Schleicher, A., Rilling, J.K., Gibson, K.R. (2003). Expansion of the neocerebellum in Hominoidea. Journal of human evolution, (44), 401-429.
Mariën, P., Engelborghs, S., Fabbro, F., & De Deyn, P. P. (2001). The lateralized linguistic cerebellum: A review and a new hypothesis. Brain and Language, 79, 580–600.
McRae, K., Hare, M., Elman, J.L., Ferretti, T. (2005). A basis for generating expectancies for verbs from nouns. Memory and Cognition, 33(7), 1174-1184.
• Deficient supervised learning in language tasks- concurrent discrimination word
learning (Fiez et al., 1992).
Middleton, F.A., and Strick, P.L. (1994). Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. Science, 266, 458-461.
Middleton, F.A., & Strick, P.L. (1998). Cerebellar output: motor and cognitive channels. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(9), 348-354.
Paulin, M.G. (1989). A Kalman filter theory of the cerebellum. In M.A. Arbib and S.-I. Amari (Eds.), Dynamic Interactions in Neural Networks: Models and Data (pp. 239259). New York: Springer.
Paulin, M. G. (1993). The role of the cerebellum in motor control and perception. Brain Behaviour Evolution. 41: 39-50
•The internal generation requirement: intact verb selection VS impaired verb
generation for posterolateral cerebellars- emulation-induced covert imitation?
(Gebhart et al., 2002)
•The neocerebellum-mirror neuron circuit in tool use: “mirror neurons in the
venral premotor cortex…may have input-output relationships with internal models
and may represent the desired state of action” (Imamizu et al., 2003: p.5466)
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2007). Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 105-110.
Ryding, E., Decety, J., Sjoholm, H., and Ingvar, D.H. (1993). Motor imagery activates the cerebellum regionally. A SPECT rCBF study with 99m Tc-HMPAO. Cognitive
Brain Research,1, 94-99.
Schmahmann J.D., & Pandya D.N. (1997). The cerebrocerebellar system. International Review of Neurobiology, 41, 31–60.
Stowe, L.A., Paans, A.M.J., Wijers, A.A., and Zwarts, F. (2004), Activations of “motor” and other non-language structures during sentence comprehension. Brain and
Language, 89, 290-299.
Townsend, D., & Bever, T.G. (2001). Sentence comprehension: The integration of habits and rules. Cambridge, MA. MIT Press.
Whiting, B.A., and R.A. Barton (2003). The evolution of the cortico-cerebellar complex in primates: anatomical connections predict patterns of correlated evolution. Journal
of human evolution ,44, 3-10.
Wilson, M., Knoblich, G. (2005) The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460-473.
Wolpert, D.M., Miall, R.C., & Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(9), 338-347.